doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00439

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2017/49:4 PP.439-449

The connecting competition effect on the orthographic-phonological connection coding for children with spelling difficulties


Abstract:
Spelling difficulty is a common phenomenon; it is one of the major types of learning disorder. An extensive amount of research has yielded considerable evidence that children with spelling difficulties have impairments in orthographic-phonological connections. However, there still remains equivocal about the mechanism of grapheme-sound connection deficits. In the present study, we aimed to investigate the competition interference effect of global phonology and radical phonology on grapheme-sound connection coding for children with spelling difficulties.
The first experiment involved artificial single characters as orthographic material, requiring all participants to remember the correspondence between the grapheme and the sound. The grapheme-sound connecting level of children with spelling difficulties was compared across two conditions: competitive phonological condition and control condition. In the competitive phonological condition, pseudo-words with high phonological accessibility were used as experimental material. In the control condition, the phonological accessibility of pseudo-words was low.
In the second experiment, artificial compound characters of left-right structure were used as experimental material. All participants were asked to remember the association between the grapheme and the sound. As the same as the first experiment, there were two different conditions: competitive phonological condition and control condition. In the competitive phonological condition, the radical phonology was different from the global phonology, which caused competition interference with global grapheme-sound connecting. In the control condition, no competition interference would be induced since the phonetic components were not into the word.
Results of the first experiment found that the children with spelling difficulty showed a lower rate of accuracy of the grapheme-sound connection than the control group, but no significant differences were found between two groups on the response time. In addition, the control group obtained significant lower accuracy in the competitive condition than in the control condition, whereas children with spelling difficulties showed no significant differences between the competitive and control conditions (neither accuracy nor response time).
Results of the second experiment indicated that the children with spelling difficulties showed a lower rate of accuracy and a longer response time than the control group. Furthermore, the response time in the competitive condition was longer than that in the control condition for the two groups, which suggested that the two groups were both effected by the competition interference, but according to the results, the effect was more significant for the children with spelling difficulties.
Generally, concerning the grapheme-sound connection coding, it was found, as in previous studies, that children with spelling difficulties performed worse than normal children. But the competition of global phonology had no effect on the grapheme-sound connecting for the children with spelling difficulties. One reason for this finding, we suspect, is that children with spelling difficulties mainly depend on visual processing, and lack sensitivity to phonological information. As a result, the phonological information of characters with similar form is hard to be automatically activated by global orthography. As for another finding that the competition interference effect of radical phonology on the grapheme-sound connection is more significant for the children with spelling difficulties, there may be two explanations: (1) children with spelling difficulties have impairments in grapheme-sound connecting, and they are easily interrupted by activated radical phonology; (2) the grapheme-sound connection representations stored in the working memory of the children with spelling difficulties are more likely to be influenced by the proactive interference of activated radical phonology.

Key words:spelling difficulties,grapheme-sound connection,interference

ReleaseDate:2017-05-12 17:01:35



Avdyli, R., & Cuetos, F. (2012). Reading difficulties in Albanian. Annals of Dyslexia, 62(3), 137-152.

Bader, R., Mecklinger, A., Hoppstadter, M., & Mayer, P. (2010). Recognition memory for one-trial-unitized word pairs: Evidence from event-related potentials. NeuroImage, 50(2), 772-781.

Burden, V. (1992). Why are some “normal” readers such poor spellers? In C. M. Sterling & C. Robson (Eds.), Psychology, spelling and education (pp. 200–214). Clevedon, U. K.: Multilingual Matters.

Cordewener, K. A. H., Bosman, A. M. T., & Verhoeven, L. (2012). Predicting early spelling difficulties in children with specific language impairment: A clinical perspective. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(6), 2279-2291.

Dries, F., Gonny, W., & Leo, B. (2011). Evidence for a specific cross-modal association deficit in dyslexia: An electrophysiological study of letter-speech sound processing. Developmental Science, 14(4), 635-648.

Fox, E. (1994). Grapheme-phoneme correspondence in dyslexic and matched control readers. British Journal of Psychology, 85(1), 41-53.

Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia surface dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Neurophysiological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 301–327). London: Erlbaum.

Fu, Z. F., Sun, X. W., & Liang, P. P. (2014). Proactive interference in episodic memory: Neuropsychopharmacology and computational cognitive modeling study. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(4), 492-499. [符仲芳, 孙洵伟, 梁佩鹏. (2014). 情节记忆中的前摄干扰: 神经心理药物学及计算认知建模研究. 心理学报, 46(4), 492-499]

Holmes, V. M., & Castles, A. E. (2001). Unexpectedly poor spelling in university students. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(4), 319-350.

Holmes, V. M., Malone, A. M., & Redenbach, H. (2008). Orthographic processing and visual sequential memory in unexpectedly poor spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 31(1), 134-156.

Holmes, V. M., & Quinn, L. (2009). Unexpectedly poor spelling and phonological-processing skill. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(4), 295-317.

Hulme, C., Goetz, K., Gooch, D., Adams, J., & Snowling, M. J. (2007). Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96(2), 150-166.

Ise, E., Arnoldi, C. J., Bartling, J., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2012). Implicit learning in children with spelling disability: Evidence from artificial grammar learning. Journal of Neural Transmission, 119(9), 999-1010.

Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience, 139(1), 181-193.

Landerl, K., & Moll, K. (2010). Comorbidity of learning disorders: Prevalence and familial transmission. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 51(3), 287-294.

Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(1), 150-161.

Larsson, M., Sandberg, A. D., & Smith, M. (2009). Reading and spelling in children with severe speech and physical impairments. Journal of Developmental & Physical Disabilities, 21(5), 369-392.

Leong, C. K. (1999). Psychological and educational aspects of specific learning disabilities. Hong Kong Journal of Paediatrics, 4, 151-159.

Li, H., & Shu, H. (2009). Paired association learning deficit: A new perspective for developmental dyslexia research. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (9), 54-58. [李虹, 舒华. (2009). 配对联想学习缺陷——探索阅读障碍的新视角. 中国特殊教育, (9), 54-58.]

Lin, M., Liu, X. P., Zhang, J. Q., & Kang, X. (2009). Language and visual processing deficits in children with Chinese reading-spelling difficulty and spelling difficulty. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 23(1), 40-43. [林敏, 刘翔平, 张婧乔, 康雪. (2009). 读写困难与听写困难儿童的言语和视知觉认知缺陷. 中国心理卫生杂志, 23(1), 40-43.]

Litt, R. A., & Nation, K. (2014). The nature and specificity of paired associate learning deficits in children with dyslexia. Journal of Memory & Language, 71(1), 71-88.

Liu, Y. Z., Yan, L. I., & Liu, X. P. (2014). Visual connection and verbal connection in children with Chinese spelling difficulties. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 28(3), 197- 201. [刘议泽, 李燕, 刘翔平. (2014). 汉语听写障碍儿童的形-形与音-音联结. 中国心理卫生杂志, 28(3), 197-201.]

Mao, R. J., Liu, X. P., & Zhang, X. (2013). An experimental study of the mental representation of Chinese characters in children with dictation disabilities. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (6), 87-91. [毛荣建, 刘翔平, 张旭. (2013). 汉字听写困难儿童字形整体表征的实验研究. 中国特殊教育, (6), 87-91.]

Mao, R. J. (2015). An experimental study of the visual processing in children with dictation disabilities. Journal of Heilongjiang College of Education, 34(5), 83-84. [毛荣建. (2015). 汉字听写困难儿童视觉加工实验研究. 黑龙江教育学院学报, 34(5), 83-84.]

Masterson, J., Laxon, V., Lovejoy, S., & Morris, V. (2007). Phonological skill, lexical decision and letter report performance in good and poor adult spellers. Journal of Research in Reading, 30, 429-442.

Mayes, A., Montaldi, D., & Migo, E. (2007). Associative memory and the medial temporal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 126-135.

Melby-Lervåg, M., Lyster, S. A. H., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 322-352.

Mo, L. (1986). An experiment about the encoding mode of short-term memory. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 18(2), 166-173. [莫雷. (1986). 关于短时记忆编码方式的实验研究. 心理学报, 18(2), 166-173.]

Moats, L. C. (1995). Spelling: Development disability and instruction. Baltimore, MD: York Press.

Moll, K., & Landerl, K. (2009). Double dissociation between reading and spelling deficits. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(5), 359-382.

Paulesu, E., Démonet, J. F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., … Frith, U. (2001). Dyslexia: Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291(5511), 2165-2167.

Purcell, J. J., Napoliello, E. M., & Eden, G. F. (2011). A combined fMRI study of typed spelling and reading. NeuroImage, 55(2), 750-762.

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126(4), 841-865.

Rothe, J., Cornell, S., Ise, E., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2015). A comparison of orthographic processing in children with and without reading and spelling disorder in a regular orthography. Reading & Writing, 28(9), 1307-1332.

Schulte-Körne, G., Bruder, J., Ise, E., & Rückert, E. (2012). Spelling disability–neurophysiologic correlates and intervention. In Z. Breznitz, O. Rubinsten, V. J. Molfese, & D. L. Molfese (Eds.), Reading, writing, mathematics and the developing brain: Listening to many voices (pp. 157–175). Netherlands: Springer.

Shu, H., Wu, N. N., Zheng, X. J., & Zhou, X. L. (1998). The phonetical representation characteristics and distribution of Chinese photo-phoneic character for elementary school children. Applied Linguistics, (2), 65-70. [舒华, 武宁宁, 郑先隽, 周晓林. (1998). 小学汉字形声字表音特点及其分布的研究. 语言文字应用, (2), 65-70.]

Snowling, M. J. (1980). The development of grapheme- phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29(2), 294- 305.

Snowling, M. J. (1995). Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 18(2), 132–138.

Spencer, K. (1999). Predicting word-spelling difficulty in 7- to 11-year-olds. Journal of Research in Reading, 22(3), 283-292.

Tilanus, E. A. T., Segers, E., & Verhoeven, L. (2016). Responsiveness to intervention in children with dyslexia. Dyslexia, 22(3), 214-232. van Weerdenburg, M., Verhoeven, L., Bosman, A., & van Balkom, H. (2011). Predicting word decoding and word spelling development in children with Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44(3), 392-411.

Wang, T. F., Li, C. H., Xu, F., Guo, X. L., & Luo, X. M. (2011). The role of associative learning and working memory in the development of fluid intelligence. Psychological Development & Education, (4), 337-343. [王腾飞, 李春花, 徐芬, 郭筱琳, 罗献明. (2011). 联想学习与工作记忆在流体智力发展中的作用. 心理发展与教育, (4), 337-343.]

Warmington, M., & Hulme, C. (2012). Phoneme awareness, visual-verbal paired-associate learning, and rapid automatized naming as predictors of individual differences in reading ability. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16(1), 45-62.

World Health Organization. (1992). International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems (Tenth Revision). Geneva (ICD-10).

Xia, X. S. (2015). An morphemic analysis of derivated Chinese characters. Journal of Yangtze University (Social Sciences), 38(1), 81-83. [夏学胜. (2015). 合体字的派生与字干、字缀的分化. 长江大学学报(社科版), 38(1), 81-83.]

Yang, S., Ning, N., & Liu, D. L. (2010). Effects of orthographical and phonological coding difficulty on the grapheme-sound connection memory of children with spelling difficulties. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (1), 31-34. [杨双, 宁宁, 刘栋梁. (2010). 图形和语音编码难度对听写困难儿童形音联结记忆的影响. 中国特殊教育, (1), 31-34.]

Yang, S., Ning, N., Liu, X. P., & Pan, Y. Z. (2009a). The characteristics of orthographic output errors by children with dysgraphia. Psychological Science, 32(2), 352-355. [杨双, 宁宁, 刘翔平, 潘益中. (2009a). 听写障碍儿童的字形输出错误特点. 心理科学, 32(2), 352-355.]

Yang, S., Ning, N., Liu, X. P., & Pan, Y. Z. (2009b). Objective-space connection memory in children with chinese spelling difficulties. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 23(6), 411-414. [杨双, 宁宁, 刘翔平, 潘益中. (2009b). 听写困难儿童对客体-空间特征的联结记忆. 中国心理卫生杂志, 23(6), 411-414.]

Yang, S., Ning, N., Liu, X. P., Pan, Y. Z., & Lu, J. (2009c). The global interruption effect on stroke processing for children with spelling difficulties. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41(2), 127-134. [杨双, 宁宁, 刘翔平, 潘益中, 卢佳. (2009c). 听写困难儿童在笔画加工中的整体干扰效应. 心理学报, 41(2), 127-134.]

Yang, S., Ning, N., Liu, X. P., Yan, Y. T., & Pan, Y. Z. (2008). The characteristics of global orthographic processing for children with spelling difficulties. Psychological Development & Education, (4), 34-38. [杨双, 宁宁, 刘翔平, 严云堂, 潘益中. (2008). 听写困难儿童的整体字形加工特点. 心理发展与教育, (4), 34-38.]

Yonelinas, A. P. (2002). The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of Memory and Language, 46(3), 441-517.

Zhang, L. N., Liu, X. P., Wu, H. J., Yang, S., & Li, C. G. (2006). Case study on grapheme-sound connection of a child with Chinese character spelling disability. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 20(12), 832-835. [张丽娜, 刘翔平, 吴洪珺, 杨双, 李成刚. (2006). 汉字听写障碍儿童形音联结个案研究. 中国心理卫生杂志, 20(12), 832-835.]

Zhang, Q. F., & Wang, C. (2014). Syllable frequency and word frequency effects in spoken and written word production in a non-alphabetic script. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 120.

Zhong, S., Liu, X. P., Li, Y., &Liu, X. D. (2011). The attentional shifting deficit of developmental dyslexia. Chinese Journal of Special Education, (3), 47-51. [钟姝, 刘翔平, 李燕, 刘晓东. (2011). 发展性阅读障碍者的注意转换缺陷研究综述. 中国特殊教育, (3), 47-51.]

Zhou, W. D. (2000). An analysis of the current chinese characters similar in form. Journal of Southwest China Normal University (Humanities and Social Sciences Edition), 26(3), 125-129. [周文德. (2000). 现行汉字形近字分析. 西南师范大学学报(人文社会科学版), 26(3), 125-129.]