doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.00460

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2017/49:4 PP.460-471

Maternal positive parenting behaviors and toddler's compliance: Prediction from maternal sensitivity in infancy and bidirectional effects of autonomy encouragement in toddlerhood


Abstract:
Children's compliance is considered as an important developmental milestone in the process of socialization and self-regulation during toddlerhood. Two forms of compliant behaviors have been distinguished in previous studies: committed compliance, with children complying to directives with willing stance; situational compliance, with children needing adult's frequent prompt to behave properly. And committed compliance but not the situational compliance were found to show its implication for internalization in preschool years. In addition, parental control strategies and power-assertive behaviors were found to be correlated to children's compliance, but only have limited explanation for individual differences in compliant behaviors. Thus, the aim of the present study was to examine whether positive parenting behaviors make contribution to early development of compliance. Among the positive parenting practices, maternal sensitivity and autonomy encouragement were highlighted in current study. According to the attachment theory, mothers who displayed adequate sensitivity to their children's needs for nutrition and security in infancy would establish secure attachment relationship with their children. These secure attached children could regulate their emotion and behaviors better than their insecure counterparts, and thus, would willingly cooperate while encountering maternal requests and prohibitions in later development. It should be noted that children's urge for autonomy is booming after 2 years old. According to self-determination theory, parents should support children's assertion for autonomy in order to attain children's obedience and cooperation during this period. Therefore, autonomy encouragement would show developmentally appropriate influence on committed compliance and help to build reciprocal relationship between mother and child in toddlerhood.
Given the above considerations, this longitudinal study was conducted to investigate whether maternal sensitivity in infancy and autonomy encouragement in toddlerhood have different impact on children's two forms of compliant behaviors in different developmental phases.
84 infants (37 boys and 47 girls) and their mothers were recruited from local communities in Beijing and included as participants in this study. When children were 6 months old (T1), their temperaments were reported by mothers, and 4 mother-child interactions were videotaped during 1.5 hours' home visit and further coded to obtain maternal sensitivity by Maternal Behavioral Q-Sort. When the children were 14 months old (T2), maternal sensitivity was recorded and then coded using the same procedure as T1. When the children were 25 months old (T3), maternal autonomy encouragement was indexed by a coding system that focuses on autonomy- or connectedness-oriented parenting behaviors during three interactions at home, and children's compliant and noncompliant behaviors were coded based on the clean-up task. At T4, the children were 38 months old and were invited to visit the laboratory for 2 hours. Maternal autonomy encouragement and children's compliance were recorded and then coded using the same procedures as T3.
As hypothesized, the results indicated that maternal sensitivity in infancy was positively associated with 2-year-olds' committed compliance at a marginally significant level and negatively associated with children's situational compliance at 2 and 3 years old. Moreover, after taking into account continuity in children's compliance and maternal autonomy encouragement during 2 years old to 3 years old, positive reciprocal influences were found between children's committed compliance and maternal autonomy encouragement, but none of the cross-lagged path coefficients was found significantly between children's situational compliance and maternal autonomy encouragement.
In summary, maternal sensitivity in infancy, as a contributor to children's secure attachment, was found to have promotive effect on children's committed compliance only at 2 years old but not at 3 years old. These findings suggest that when the toddlers are at an age struggling for self-achievement and self-confidence, it is important to highlight maternal autonomy encouragement as another developmentally appropriate parenting practice. Moreover, this longitudinal study further provided evidence for maternal autonomy encouragement as a positive parenting practice for cultivating children's committed compliance during 2 to 3 years old of age. That is, during toddlerhood, mothers who encourage their children to be autonomous would find their children more cooperative and compliant, which, in turn, prompts mothers to give more autonomy to their children. Thus, by adjusting their positive parenting behaviors from sensitivity to autonomy encouragement when children are during the transition from infancy to toddlerhood, parents will exhibit positive influence on children's committed compliance. The findings of this study suggest that if parenting practices interventions are successful at matching children's needs in different developmental phases, this may help cultivate children's committed compliance. That is, for the development of children's compliance to proceed well, parents should have adequate sensitivity to their children's needs for nutrition and security in infancy, and give their children more opportunities to regulate behaviors in daily lives with increase of need for autonomy in toddlerhood.

Key words:compliance,positive parenting,developmentally appropriate practice,maternal sensitivity,autonomy encouragement,developmental needs

ReleaseDate:2017-05-12 17:01:36



Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc..

Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 81–95.

Blandon, A. Y., & Volling, B. L. (2008). Parental gentle guidance and children's compliance within the family: A replication study. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(3), 355–366.

Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Attachment (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published in 1969)

Carey, W. B., & McDevitt, S. C. (1978). Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61(5), 735–739.

Chen, H. C., Li, D. H., Hou, J., & Chen, X. Y. (2003). Maternal control strategies and the child's compliant behavior in family free play. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 35(2), 209–215. [陈会昌, 李冬晖, 侯静, 陈欣银. (2003). 家庭游戏中的母亲控制策略与儿童顺从行为. 心理学报, 35(2), 209–215.]

Chen, H. Y., & Little, R. (1999). A test of missing completely at random for generalised estimating equations with missing data. Biometrika, 86(1), 1–13.

Chen, X. Y., Rubin, K. H., Liu, M. W., Chen, H. C., Wang, L., Li, D.,... Li, B. S. (2003). Compliance in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(5), 428–436.

Crockenberg, S., & Litman, C. (1990). Autonomy as competence in 2-year-olds: Maternal correlates of child defiance, compliance, and self-assertion. Developmental Psychology, 26(6), 961–971.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “What” and “Why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Enquiry, 11(4), 227–268.

Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 3–25.

Dix, T., Stewart, A. D., Gershoff, E. T., & Day, W. H. (2007). Autonomy and children's reactions to being controlled: Evidence that both compliance and defiance may be positive markers in early development. Child Development, 78(4), 1204–1221.

Feldman, R., & Klein, P. S. (2003). Toddlers' self-regulated compliance to mothers, caregivers, and fathers: Implications for theories of socialization. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 680–692.

Grusec, J. E. (2011). Socialization processes in the family: Social and emotional development. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 243–269.

Grusec, J. E., & Davidov, M. (2010). Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development, 81(3), 687–709.

Grusec, J. E., & Goodnow, J. J. (1994). Impact of parental discipline methods on the child's internalization of values: A reconceptualization of current points of view. Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 4–19.

Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Conscience, personality, and socialization techniques. Human Development, 13, 90–126.

Holden, G. W. (2015). Parenting: A dynamic perspective (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications.

Huang, C. Y., & Lamb, M. E. (2014). Are Chinese children more compliant? Examination of the cultural difference in observed maternal control and child compliance. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(4), 507–533.

Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2008). Methods of the video-feedback programs to promote positive parenting alone, with sensitive discipline, and with representational attachment discussions. In F. Juffer, M. J. Bakermans-Kranenburg, & M. H. van IJzendoorn (Eds.), Promoting positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention (pp. 11–22). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Kochanska, G. (2002). Mutually responsive orientation between mothers and their young children: A context for the early development of conscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(6), 191–195.

Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (1995). Mother-child mutually positive affect, the quality of child compliance to requests and prohibitions, and maternal control as correlates of early internalization. Child Development, 66(1), 236–254.

Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., & Carlson, J. J. (2005). Temperament, relationships, and young children's receptive cooperation with their parents. Developmental Psychology, 41(4), 648–660.

Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The development of self-regulation in the first four years of life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091–1111.

Kochanska, G., & Kim, S. (2013). Difficult temperament moderates links between maternal responsiveness and children's compliance and behavior problems in low- income families. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(3), 323–332.

Kochanska, G., & Kuczynski, L. (1991). Maternal autonomy granting: Predictors of normal and depressed mothers' compliance and noncompliance with the requests of five-year-olds. Child Development, 62(6), 1449–1459.

Kochanska, G., & Murray, K. T. (2000). Mother–child mutually responsive orientation and conscience development: From toddler to early school age. Child Development, 71(2), 417–431.

Kok, R., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Linting, M., Bakermans- Kranenburg, M. J., Tharner, A., Luijk, M. P. C. M., … Tiemeier, H. (2013). Attachment insecurity predicts child active resistance to parental requests in a compliance task. Child: Care, Health and Development, 39(2), 277–287.

Liang, Y. H., & Pang, L. J. (2011). Developmentally appropriate practice studies: Content, effect and trend. Global Education, 40(8), 53–59. [梁玉华, 庞丽娟. (2011). 发展适宜性教育: 内涵、效果及其趋势. 全球教育展望, 40(8), 53–59.]

Lin, Q., Wang, Z. Y., Lu, S., Liang, X., He, Q., Wang, Z., & Hu, R. S. (2014). Internal working models of toddlers: A bridge from maternal sensitivity to toddlers' attachment behaviors. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 46(3), 353–366. [林青, 王争艳, 卢珊, 梁熙, 贺琼, 王朝, 胡若时. (2014). 从母亲的敏感性到学步儿的依恋安全性: 内部工作模式的桥梁作用. 心理学报, 46(3), 353–366.]

Liu, M. W., Chen, X. Y., Zheng, S. J., Chen, H. C., & Wang, L. (2009). Maternal autonomy- and connectedness-oriented parenting behaviors as predictors of children's social behaviors in China. Social Development, 18(3), 671–689.

Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child interaction. In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4., pp. 1–102). New York: Wiley.

Moran, G., Pederson, D. R., & Tarabulsy, G. M. (2011). Becoming sensitive to sensitivity: Lessons learned from the development of the maternal behavior Q-sort. In D. W. Davis & C. Logdson (Eds.), Maternal sensitivity: A critical review for practitioners. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Publishers.

Newton, E. K., Laible, D., Carlo, G., Steele, J. S., & McGinley, M. (2014). Do sensitive parents foster kind children, or vice versa? Bidirectional influences between children's prosocial behavior and parental sensitivity. Developmental Psychology, 50(6), 1808–1816.

Oliphant, A. E., & Kuczynski, L. (2011). Mothers' and fathers' perceptions of mutuality in middle childhood: The domain of intimacy. Journal of Family Issues, 32(8), 1104–1124.

Parpal, M., & Maccoby, E. E. (1985). Maternal responsiveness and subsequent child compliance. Child Development, 56(5), 1326–1334.

Patterson, G. R. (1986). Performance models for antisocial boys. American Psychologist, 41(4), 432–444.

Pederson, D. R., Moran G., & Bento, S. (2009). Assessing maternal sensitivity and the quality of mother-infant interactions using “The Maternal Behavior Q-Sort (MBQS)”. Pederson-Moran MBQS manual. (unpublished manuscript). Western University at Ontario, Canada.

Roskam, I. (2015). Enhancing positive parenting through metacognition with the program “Lou & us”. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 24(8), 2496–2507.

Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological needs and the facilitation of integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63(3), 397–427.

Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2015). Autonomy and autonomy disturbances in self-development and psychopathology: Research on motivation, attachment, and clinical process. In D. Chicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Theory and method (Vol. 1, pp. 795–849). New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Silva, K. M., Eggum, N. D., Reiser, M., Edwards, A., …, Gaertner, B. M. (2012). Longitudinal relations among maternal behaviors, effortful control and young children's committed compliance. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 552–566.

Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E. A., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford.

Stayton, D. J., Hogan, R., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1971). Infant obedience and maternal behavior: The origins of socialization reconsidered. Child Development, 42(4), 1057–1069.

Thompson, R. A. (2008). Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 348–365). New York: Guilford.

Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation in the family. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 249–268). New York: Guilford.

Ye, S. J., Tang, W. Q., Zhang, M. Q., & Cao, W. C. (2014). Techniques for missing data in longitudinal studies and its application. Advances in Psychological Science, 22(12), 1985–1994. [叶素静, 唐文清, 张敏强, 曹魏聪. (2014). 追踪研究中缺失数据处理方法及应用现状分析. 心理科学进展, 22(12), 1985–1994.]

Zhang, J. S., Xu, J. D., & Shen, L. X. (2000). The assessment of Carey's five temperament questionnaires. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 14(3), 153–156. [张劲松, 许积德, 沈理笑. (2000). Carey的1个月~12岁儿童气质系列问卷的应用评价. 中国心理卫生杂志, 14(3), 153–156.]

Zhang, P., Liang, Z. B., Chen, H. C., & Zhang, G. Z. (2012). The stability and gender difference of children's self-control from 2 to 11 years old. Psychological Development and Education, 28(5), 463–470.[张萍, 梁宗保, 陈会昌, 张光珍. (2012). 2~11岁儿童自我控制发展的稳定性与变化及其性别差异. 心理发展与教育, 28(5), 463–470.]