doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.01256

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2017/49:10 PP.1256-1266

The effect of perceptual load on processing and memorizing negative facial distractor


Abstract:
The processing of facial expression has been well studied. However, how facial expression is processed and memorized when it is presented outside the focus of attention is little known. Previous results of negative distractor processing were contradicted. Some studies found that processing of emotional distractor is resistant to perceptual load, whereas other studies found that emotional distractor processing is modulated by perceptual load. Interestingly, two recent studies have shown that the processing of emotional charged distractor are both automatically and depending on perceptual load. However, the temporal information of automatically and load depending processing of emotional charged distractor, is little known. Whether the individual can detect a distractor during early phase automatically, and suppress the distractor under high perceptual load at late phase of processing. To this end, we investigate how perceptual load affects facial expression distractor processing at different phase of encoding by using event-related potential methods. In ERP studies, the N170 and N250 are two early components which sensitive to the attentional resources and facial expression, while the N700 is a late component which related to saving stimulus into long-term memory when stimulus is disappear. According to previous studies and perceptual load theory, we hypothesized that facial expression distractor would process automatically in the early phase, while it modulates by perceptual load in the late phase. We expect that N170 and N250 would modulate by distractor valence but not perceptual load, while N700 would modulate by the interaction between perceptual load and distractor valence.
Participants were asked to perform a letter search task which adopted from (Jenkins et al., 2005), followed by a surprise recognition task for facial expression distractor. In the letter search task, a letter string superposed on a facial distractor, participants were required to detect specific character and make response. Perceptual load (low, high) and distractor valence (neutral, fearful) were manipulated. In the surprise recognition task, participants were asked to judge whether the face was presented in the letter search task, half of the faces were presented in the letter search task, half of the faces were not presented before.
In the letter search task, behavioral results showed that fearful distractor, relative to neutral distractor, interfere with visual search performance in the high perceptual load condition, while this interference effect was not significant in low perceptual load condition. ERP results showed that fearful distractor, relative to neutral distractor, elicit larger N170 and N250. The N170 and N250 were not modulated by perceptual load. In contrast, the N700 which related to post-stimuli processing and learning was modulated by the interaction between perceptual load and distractor valence. Fearful distractor elicited larger N700 than neutral distractor under low perceptual load, whereas there was no significant difference between fearful and neutral distractor under high perceptual load. The memory performance was mimic the N700 results, participant recognized fearful distractor better than neutral distractor under low load, whereas there was no significant difference under high load.
These results suggest that the processing of facial expression distractor affected by perceptual load at different stage during encoding phase. In the early stage, the facial distractor is process automatically, whereas in the late stage, the post-stimuli processing is modulated by perceptual load and valence. Low perceptual load leads to less filtering of task irrelevant negative information, which induced a memory bias for negative distractor.

Key words:fearful expression,perceptual load,N170,N700

ReleaseDate:2017-11-17 09:47:11



Attar, H. C., Andersen, S. K., & Müller, M. M. (2010). Time course of affective bias in visual attention:Convergent evidence from steady-state visual evoked potentials and behavioral data. NeuroImage, 53, 1326-1333.

Attar, H. C., & Müller, M. M. (2012). Selective attention to task-irrelevant emotional distractors is unaffected by the perceptual load associated with a foreground task. PLoS One, 7, e37186.

Bender, S., Behringer, S., Freitag, C. M., Resch, F., & Weisbrod, M. (2010). Transmodal comparison of auditory, motor, and visual post-processing with and without intentional short-term memory maintenance. Clinical Neurophysiology, 121, 2044-2064.

Bender, S., Oelkers-Ax, R., Hellwig, S., Resch, F., & Weisbrod, M. (2008). The topography of the scalp-recorded visual N700. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119, 587-604.

Bishop, S. J., Jenkins, R., & Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Neural processing of fearful faces:Effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1595-1603.

Brosch, T., Sander, D., Pourtois, G., & Scherer, K. R. (2008). Beyond fear:Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychological Science, 19, 362-370.

Calvo, M. G., & Beltrán, D. (2013). Recognition advantage of happy faces:Tracing the neurocognitive processes. Neuropsychologia, 51, 2051-2061.

Calvo, M. G., & Beltrán, D. (2014). Brain lateralization of holistic versus analytic processing of emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage, 92, 237-247.

Cohen, N., & Henik, A. (2012). Do irrelevant emotional stimuli impair or improve executive control? Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 33.

Crist, R. E., Wu, C.-T., Karp, C., & Woldorff, M. G. (2007). Face processing is gated by visual spatial attention. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 1, 10.

Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193-222.

Dolcos, F., Wang, L., & Mather, M. (2014). Current research and emerging directions in emotion-cognition interactions. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 8, 83.

Eimer, M., & Kiss, M. (2010). An electrophysiological measure of access to representations in visual working memory. Psychophysiology, 47, 197-200.

Fan, C., Chen, S. S., Zhang, L. C., Qi, Z. Y., Jin, Y. L., Wang, Q., … Luo, W. B. (2015). N170 changes reflect competition between faces and identifiable characters during early visual processing. NeuroImage, 110, 32-38.

Gainotti, G. (2012). Unconscious processing of emotions and the right hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 50, 205-218.

Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 46-48.

Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., & Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 4115-4120.

Gupta, R., Hur, Y. J., & Lavie, N. (2016). Distracted by pleasure:Effects of positive versus negative valence on emotional capture under load. Emotion, 16, 328-337.

Hecht, M., Thiemann, U., Freitag, C. M., & Bender, S. (2016). Time-resolved neuroimaging of visual short term memory consolidation by post-perceptual attention shifts. NeuroImage, 125, 964-977.

Jenkins, R., Lavie, N., & Driver, J. (2005). Recognition memory for distractor faces depends on attentional load at exposure. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 314-320.

Jessen, S., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Neural signatures of conscious and unconscious emotional face processing in human infants. Cortex, 64, 260-270.

Kaufmann, J. M., Schweinberger, S. R., & Burton, A. M. (2009). N250 ERP correlates of the acquisition of face representations across different images. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 625-641.

Lavie, N. (2005). Distracted and confused?:Selective attention under load. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 75-82.

Lavie, N. (2010). Attention, distraction, and cognitive control under load. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19, 143-148.

Lavie, N., Ro, T., & Russell, C. (2003). The role of perceptual load in processing distractor faces. Psychological Science, 14, 510-515.

Luo, Q., Holroyd, T., Majestic, C., Cheng, X., Schechter, J., & Blair, R. J. (2010). Emotional automaticity is a matter of timing. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 5825-5829.

Mangun, G. R., Buonocore, M. H., Girelli, M., & Jha, A. P. (1998). ERP and fMRI measures of visual spatial selective attention. Human Brain Mapping, 6, 383-389.

Minamoto, T., Shipstead, Z., Osaka, N., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Low cognitive load strengthens distractor interference while high load attenuates when cognitive load and distractor possess similar visual characteristics. Attention Perception & Psychophysics, 77, 1659-1673.

Mothes-Lasch, M., Mentzel, H. J., Miltner, W. H. R., & Straube, T. (2011). Visual attention modulates brain activation to angry voices. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 9594-9598.

Müller-Bardorff, M., Schulz, C., Peterburs, J., Bruchmann, M., Mothes-Lasch, M., Miltner, W., & Straube, T. (2016). Effects of emotional intensity under perceptual load:An event-related potentials (ERPs) study. Biological Psychology, 117, 141-149.

Nasr, S., & Esteky, H. (2009). A study of N250 event-related brain potential during face and non-face detection tasks. Journal of Vision, 9, 5.1-14.

Norberg, J., Peira, N., & Wiens, S. (2010). Never mind the spider:Late positive potentials to phobic threat at fixation are unaffected by perceptual load. Psychophysiology, 47, 1151-1158.

Park, G., van Bavel, J. J., Vasey, M. W., & Thayer, J. F. (2013). Cardiac vagal tone predicts attentional engagement to and disengagement from fearful faces. Emotion, 13, 645-656.

Parks, N. A., Beck, D. M., & Kramer, A. F. (2013). Enhancement and suppression in the visual field under perceptual load. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 275.

Pessoa, L. (2009). How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 160-166.

Pessoa, L. (2015). Precis on the cognitive-emotional brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e71.

Pessoa, L., Padmala, S., & Morland, T. (2005). Fate of unattended fearful faces in the amygdala is determined by both attentional resources and cognitive modulation. Neuroimage, 28, 249-255.

Pessoa, L., McKenna, M., Gutierrez, E., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 11458-11463.

Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 619-633.

Prete, G., D'Ascenzo, S., Laeng, B., Fabri, M., Foschi, N., & Tommasi, L. (2015). Conscious and unconscious processing of facial expressions:Evidence from two split-brain patients. Journal of Neuropsychology, 9, 45-63.

Qin, S. Z., Rijpkema, M., Tendolkar, I., Piekema, C., Hermans, E. J., Binder, M., … Fernández, G. (2009). Dissecting medial temporal lobe contributions to item and associative memory formation. Neuroimage, 46, 874-881.

Righi, S., Marzi, T., Toscani, M., Baldassi, S., Ottonello, S., & Viggiano, M. P. (2012). Fearful expressions enhance recognition memory:Electrophysiological evidence. Acta Psychologica, 139, 7-18.

Rossion, B. (2014). Understanding face perception by means of human electrophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 310-318.

Rossion, B., & Caharel, S. (2011). ERP evidence for the speed of face categorization in the human brain:Disentangling the contribution of low-level visual cues from face perception. Vision Research, 51, 1297-1311.

Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q. Y., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., … Jeffries, J.. (2011). Emotional perception:Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage, 54, 2524-2533.

Sand, A., & Wiens, S. (2011). Processing of unattended, simple negative pictures resists perceptual load. Neuroreport, 22, 348-352.

Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2011). Active suppression of distractors that match the contents of visual working. Visual Cognition, 19, 956-972.

Schupp, H. T., Flaisch, T., Stockburger, J., & Junghöfer, M. (2006). Emotion and attention:Event-related brain potential studies. Progress in Brain Research, 156, 31-51.

Shafer, A. T., & Dolcos, F. (2012). Neural correlates of opposing effects of emotional distraction on perception and episodic memory:An event-related FMRI investigation. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 70.

Shafer, A. T., Matveychuk, D., Penney, T., O'Hare, A. J., Stokes, J., & Dolcos, F. (2012). Processing of emotional distraction is both automatic and modulated by attention:Evidence from an event-related fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 1233-1252.

Stefanics, G., Csukly, G., Komlósi, S., Czobor, P., & Czigler, I. (2012). Processing of unattended facial emotions:A visual mismatch negativity study. NeuroImage, 59, 3042-3049.

Thoma, V., & Lavie, N. (2013). Perceptual load effects on processing distractor faces indicate face-specific capacity limits. Visual Cognition, 21, 1053-1076.

Vuilleumier, P. (2015). Precision about the automatic emotional brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e89.

Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Effects of attention and emotion on face processing in the human brain:An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 30, 829-841.

Vuilleumier, P., & Huang, Y.-M. (2009). Emotional attention:Uncovering the mechanisms of affective biases in perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 148-152.

Wang, Y., & Luo, Y. J. (2005). Standardization and assessment of college students' facial expression of emotion. Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 13, 396-398.

[王妍, 罗跃嘉. (2005). 大学生面孔表情材料的标准化及其评定. 中国临床心理学杂志, 13, 396-398.]

Xia, M., Li, X. M., Ye, C., & Li, H. (2014). The ERPs for the facial expression processing. Advances in Psychological Science, 22, 1556-1563.

[侠牧, 李雪榴, 叶春, 李红. (2014). 面部表情加工的ERP成分. 心理科学进展, 22, 1556-1563.]

Yates, A., Ashwin, C., & Fox, E. (2010). Does emotion processing require attention? The effects of fear conditioning and perceptual load. Emotion, 10, 822-830.