doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2017.01460

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2017/49:11 PP.1460-1472

Priming effects of virtual avatars on aggression: Influence of violence and player gender


Abstract:
A virtual avatar is a video game player's self-presentation in virtual space. The physical appearance of an avatar can prime stereotypes and behavioral scripts stored in memory. The relation between avatar appearance and aggression has been substantially confirmed, but there are open questions about the conditions in which this association is strongest, and what the relation between avatar identification and aggression is. This study used a cue-priming paradigm in two experiments to test the effect of avatar appearance on avatar identification and aggression in violent and nonviolent video games; to test gender as a moderator of these effects; and to test the correlation between avatar identification and aggression.
The first experiment investigated the effect of avatar appearance on the level of avatar identification and aggression in violent and nonviolent video games. This experiment employed a 2 (Avatar Appearance:justice/evil)×2 (Game Violence:violent/non-violent) between-subjects design. 75 male participants were randomly assigned to play a violent or non-violent video game using an avatar representing justice or evil. Based on the first experiment, the second experiment explored the interaction effect of avatar appearance and gender on the level of avatar identification and aggression in a violent video game. This experiment also employed a 2 (Avatar Appearance:justice/evil)×2 (Gender:male/female) between-subjects design. 42 male and 36 female participants were randomly assigned to play a violent video game using an avatar representing justice or evil. After game play, the amount of hot sauce given by participants to an ostensible partner who hated spicy food was used to measure aggression, and an avatar identification scale was used to measure identification with the avatar.
The results showed that:(1) The relations between avatar appearance, avatar identification and aggression were influenced by the violence of game. In the violent video game, the identification with the justice avatar was significantly higher than with the evil avatar, and the evil avatar elicited significantly higher aggression than the justice avatar. In the non-violent game, there was a marginally significant difference between the identification with the justice or evil avatar, but there was no significant difference between the level of aggression elicited by the justice or evil avatar. (2) In the violent video game, the avatar effects were moderated by player gender. Specifically, the avatar identification of female participants was significantly affected by avatar appearance, whereas that of the male participant was not. Avatar appearance had a stronger impact on the aggression of males than females. (3) There was a significant correlation between avatar identification and aggression, which was moderated by game violence and gender.
In conclusion, the results of this study supported the priming effect theory and were partially consistent with the existing research. Several factors influenced the effect of avatar appearance on aggression, including a video game factor (violent or non-violent game) as well as an individual factor (male or female), and the complex relationship between avatar identification and aggression. One social implication of the study is that game designers should embed more positive associations, situations, and stereotypes in games to provide users more positive potential priming effects.

Key words:priming effect,avatar appearance,game violence,player gender,avatar identification,aggression

ReleaseDate:2017-12-29 15:49:12



Adachi, P. J. C., & Willoughby, T. (2011). The effect of video game competition and violence on aggressive behavior:Which characteristic has the greatest influence? Psychology of Violence, 1, 259-274.

Anderson, C. A., & Carnagey, N. L. (2009). Causal effects of violent sports video games on aggression:Is it competitiveness or violent content? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 731-739.

Anderson, C. A., & Ford, C. M. (1986). Affect of the game player:Short-term effects of highly and mildly aggressive video games. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12, 390-402.

Anderson, C. A., & Murphy, C. R. (2003). Violent video games and aggressive behavior in young women. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 423-429.

Anderson, C. A., Shibuya, A., Ihori, N., Swing, E. L., Bushman, B. J., Sakamoto, A., … Saleem, M. (2010). Violent video game effects on aggression, empathy, and prosocial behavior in Eastern and Western countries:A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 151-173.

Bargh, J. D., & Chartrand, T. (2000). The mind in the middle:A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253-285). New York, USA:Cambridge University Press.

Barlett, C. P., Harris, R. J., & Bruey, C. (2008). The effect of the amount of blood in a violent video game on aggression, hostility, and arousal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 539-546.

Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1-62). New York, USA:Academic Press.

Bian, Y. L., Han, L., Zhou, C., Chen, Y. M., & Gao, F. Q. (2015). The Proteus effect in virtual reality social environments:Influence of situation and shyness. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47, 363-374.[卞玉龙, 韩磊, 周超, 陈英敏, 高峰强. (2015). 虚拟现实社交环境中的普罗透斯效应:情境、羞怯的影响. 心理学报, 47, 363-374.]

Bushman, B. J., & Huesmann, L. R. (2006). Short-term and long-term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160, 348-352.

Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 452-459.

Carnagey, N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2004). Violent video game exposure and aggression:A literature review. Minerva Psichiatrica, 45, 1-18.

Carnagey, N. L., & Anderson, C. A. (2005). The effects of reward and punishment in violent video games on aggressive affect, cognition, and behavior. Psychological Science, 16, 882-889.

Cohen, J. (2006). Audience identification with media characters. In J. Bryant & P. Vorderer (Eds.), Psychology of entertainment (pp. 183-97). Mahwah, NJ, USA:Lawrence Erlbaum.

Fischer, P., Kastenmüller, A., & Greitemeyer, T. (2010). Media violence and the self:The impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 192-195.

Gentile, D. A., Lynch, P. J., Linder, J. R., & Walsh, D. A. (2004). The effects of violent video game habits on adolescent hostility, aggressive behaviors, and school performance. Journal of Adolescence, 27, 5-22.

Greitemeyer, T. (2013). Effects of playing video games on perceptions of one's humanity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 153, 499-514.

Greitemeyer, T., & Mügge, D. O. (2014). Video games do affect social outcomes:A meta-analytic review of the effects of violent and prosocial video game play. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 578-589.

Happ, C., Melzer, A., & Steffgen, G. (2013). Superman vs. BAD Man? The effects of empathy and game character in violent video games. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 16, 774-778.

Hartmann, T., & Vorderer, P. (2010). It's okay to shoot a character:Moral disengagement in violent video games. Journal of Communication, 60, 94-119.

Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization:An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 252-264.

Hollingdale, J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2013). The changing face of aggression:The effect of personalized avatars in a violent video game on levels of aggressive behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 1862-1868.

Huesmann, L. R. (1998). The role of social information processing and cognitive schema in the acquisition and maintenance of habitual aggressive behavior. In R. G. Geen & E. Donnerstein (Eds.), Human aggression:Theories, research, and implications for social policy (pp. 73-109). New York, USA:Academic Press.

Huesmann, L. R., Moise-Titus, J., Podolski, C. P., & Eron, L. D. (2003). Longitudinal relations between childhood exposure to media violence and adult aggression and violence:1977-1992. Developmental Psychology, 39, 201-221.

Klimmt, C., Hefner, D.,& Vorderer, P. (2009). The video game experience as "true" identification:A theory of enjoyable alterations of players' self-perception. Communication Theory, 19, 351-373.

Konijn, E. A., Bijvank, M. N., & Bushman, B. J. (2007). I wish I were a warrior:The role of wishful identification in the effects of violent video games on aggression in adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1038-1044.

Li, X. Y., Fei, L. P., Zhang, Y. L., Niu, Y. J., Tong, Y. S., & Yang, S. J. (2011). Development, reliability and validity of the Chinese version of Buss & Perry aggression questionnaire. Chinese Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 37, 607-613.[李献云, 费立鹏, 张亚利, 牛雅娟, 童永胜, 杨少杰. (2011). Buss和Perry攻击问卷中文版的修订和信效度. 中国神经精神疾病杂志, 37, 607-613.]

Lieberman, J. D., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & McGregor, H. A. (1999). A hot new way to measure aggression:Hot sauce allocation. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 331-348.

Lin, J.-H. (2013a). Do video games exert stronger effects on aggression than film? The role of media interactivity and identification on the association of violent content and aggressive outcomes. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 535-543.

Lin, J.-H. (2013b). Identification matters:A moderated mediation model of media interactivity, character identification, and video game violence on aggression. Journal of Communication, 63, 682-702.

Lin, S. F. (2010). Gender differences and the effect of contextual features on game enjoyment and responses. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 13, 533-537.

Nelson, L. D., & Norton, M. I. (2005). From student to superhero:Situational primes shape future helping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 423-430.

Peña, J. F. (2011). Integrating the influence of perceiving and operating avatars under the automaticity model of priming effects. Communication Theory, 21, 150-168.

Peña, J. F, Hancock, J. T., & Merola, N. A. (2009). The priming effects of avatars in virtual settings. Communication Research, 36, 838-856.

Saleem, M., Anderson, C. A., & Gentile, D. A. (2012). Effects of prosocial, neutral, and violent video games on college students' affect. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 263-271.

Shapiro, M. A., Peña-Herborn, J., & Hancock, J. T. (2006). Realism, imagination and narrative video games. In P. Vorderer & J. Bryant (Eds.), Playing video games:Motives, responses, and consequences (pp. 275-289). Mahwah, NJ, USA:Lawrence Erlbaum.

Soutter, A. R. B., & Hitchens, M. (2016). The relationship between character identification and flow state within video Games. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 1030-1038.

Teng, Z. J., Liu, Y. L., & Guo, C. (2015). The debate of violent video game impact on aggression. Psychological Development and Education, 31, 494-502.[滕召军, 刘衍玲, 郭成. (2015). 暴力电子游戏对攻击行为的影响及其争论. 心理发展与教育, 31, 494-502.]

Trepte, S., & Reinecke, L. (2010). Avatar creation and video game enjoyment:Effects of life-satisfaction, game competitiveness, and identification with the avatar. Journal of Media Psychology, 22, 171-184.

Turkay, S., & Kinzer, C. K. (2014). The effects of avatar:Based customization on player identification. International Journal of Gaming and Computer-Mediated Simulations, 6, 1-25.

van Looy, J., Courtois, C., De Vocht, M., & De Marez, L. (2012). Player identification in online games:Validation of a scale for measuring identification in MMOGs. Media Psychology, 15, 197-221.

Webster, G. D., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2006). Behavioral and self-reported aggression as a function of domain-specific self-esteem. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 17-27.

Yang, G. S., Gibson, B., Lueke, A. K., Huesmann, L. R., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Effects of avatar race in violent video games on racial attitudes and aggression. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 698-704.

Yang, G. S., Huesmann, L. R., & Bushman, B. J. (2014). Effects of playing a violent video game as male versus female avatar on subsequent aggression in male and female players. Aggressive Behavior, 40, 537-541.

Yee, N., & Bailenson, J. N. (2007). The Proteus effect:The effect of transformed self-representation on behavior. Human Communication Research, 33, 271-290.

Yee, N., Bailenson, J. N., & Ducheneaut, N. (2009). The Proteus effect:Implications of transformed digital self-representation on online and offline behavior. Communication Research, 36, 285-312.

Yoon, G., & Vargas, P. T. (2014). Know thy avatar:The unintended effect of virtual-self representation on behavior. Psychological Science, 25, 1043-1045.

Zhang, X. M., Li, M., Song, Y., Li, Y. N., & Wei, L. Q. (2009). The effect of the violent elements in violent games on the players and the observers' aggression. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 41, 1228-1236.[张学民, 李茂, 宋艳, 李永娜, 魏柳青. (2009). 暴力游戏中射杀动作和血腥成分对玩家和观看者攻击倾向的影响. 心理学报, 41, 1228-1236.]