doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00270

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2018/50:3 PP.270-282

The effect of perceptual load on the multiple social categories processing of faces


Abstract:
Perceptual load theory claims that the information processing stream of task-irrelevant stimuli would fade away along with the increase of load levels. This view has been confirmed when words, objects, celebrities and animal faces were regarded as task-irrelevant stimuli. But there is still a debate as to whether perceptual load levels could affect the processing of race and gender which belong to primary social categories of human faces.
For this purpose, a new name judgement task was developed based on the flanker paradigm. Each trial consisted of the following sequence of events, a fixation cross that was present for 500 ms was followed by a vertically arranged names matrix in the center of screen, and the interfering face randomly appeared on the left or the right side of the screen simultaneously. The participants were asked to judge the race or gender of the only real name presented in each matrix. This stimulus did not disappear until the participants pressed a task key. The inter-trial interval (ITI) was 1000 ms long.
Current study consisted of two experiments. Experiment 1 explored the interference effect of face on the race judgement of the real name at different load levels. Twenty-six undergraduate students were recruited in experiment 1. The results indicated that the higher the perceptual load level was, the longer of RT was. Moreover, RT of congruence condition, in which the race of real name is identical with that of adjacent face, was always shorter than that of incongruence condition. The results indicated that the race judgment of real names was always affected by the image of face. Experiment 2a-a conceptual replication-investigated the interference effect of the face on the gender judgement of real name at different load levels. We recruited thirty-three undergraduate students as participants and the results showed that the interference effect of face on the gender judgement of real name disappeared and there was no significant difference on RT between the congruence and the incongruence conditions at the high load levels. Experiment 2b (N=26)-a conceptual extension-investigated the interference effect of the face on Chinese names with the neutral feature. The results indicated that at the low load levels, the gender judgment of neutral names was affected by image of face. However, at the high load levels, the accuracy rate of judging the gender of neutral name was at a random level.
These results suggested that the attention system might have a bottom-up obligatory processing characteristic for racial information, whereas there was a top-down processing characteristic with a certain degree of flexibility on gender information which was modulated by the attention resource. In conclusion, results from the current study reveals that the perceptual load theory has certain limitations to explain face primary social categories processing. It is also indirectly proved that culture and theory of evolution have a profound effect on social categories processing of human.

Key words:perceptual load,race,gender,obligatory processing,flexible processing

ReleaseDate:2018-03-29 16:23:27



Bruce, V., & Young, A. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77(3), 305-327.

Campbell, R., Benson, P. J., Wallace, S. B., Doesbergh, S., & Coleman, M. (1999). More about brows:How poses that change brow position affect perceptions of gender. Perception, 28(4), 489-504.

Dailey, M. N., Joyce, C., Lyons, M. J., Kamachi, M., Ishi, H., Gyoba, J., & Cottrell, G. W. (2010). Evidence and a computational explanation of cultural differences in facial expression recognition. Emotion, 10(6), 874-893.

de Frias, C. M., Nilsson, L.-G., & Herlitz, A. (2006). Sex differences in cognition are stable over a 10-year period in adulthood and old age. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13(3-4), 574-587.

Donders, N. C., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2008). Danger stereotypes predict racially biased attentional allocation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 1328-1333.

Freeman, J. B., Ambady, N., & Holcomb, P. J. (2010). The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information. Neuroreport, 21(1), 24-28.

Fukui, Y., Yamashita, T., Hishikawa, N., Kurata, T., Sato, K., Omote, Y.,... Abe, K. (2015). Computerized touch-panel screening tests for detecting mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Internal Medicine, 54(8), 895-902.

Goshen-Gottstein, Y., & Ganel, T. (2000). Repetition priming for familiar and unfamiliar faces in a sex-judgment task:evidence for a common route for the processing of sex and identity. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1198-1214.

Gupta, R., & Srinivasan, N. (2015). Only irrelevant sad but not happy faces are inhibited under high perceptual load. Cognition and Emotion, 29(4), 747-754.

Hügelschäfer, S., Jaudas, A., & Achtziger, A. (2016). Detecting gender before you know it:How implementation intentions control early gender categorization. Brain Research, 1649, 9-22.

Hains, P., & Baillargeon, J. (2011). La face animale et le visage humain sont-ils équivalents? Une étude dans le cadre de la théorie de la charge perceptuelle. L'Année Psychologique, 111(3), 449-463.

Herlitz, A., & Lovén, J. (2013). Sex differences and the own-gender bias in face recognition:A meta-analytic review. Visual Cognition, 21(9-10), 1306-1336.

Herlitz, A., Reuterskiöld, L., Lovén, J., Thilers, P. P., & Rehnman, J. (2013). Cognitive sex differences are not magnified as a function of age, sex hormones, or puberty development during early adolescence. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(3), 167-179.

Herzmann, G., Schweinberger, S. R., Sommer, W., & Jentzsch, I. (2004). What's special about personally familiar faces? A multimodal approach. Psychophysiology, 41(5), 688-701.

Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2003). Race and gender on the brain:Electrocortical measures of attention to the race and gender of multiply categorizable individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(4), 616-626.

Ito, T. A., & Urland, G. R. (2005). The influence of processing objectives on the perception of faces:An ERP study of race and gender perception. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5(1), 21-36.

Jung, K., Ruthruff, E., & Gaspelin, N. (2013). Automatic identification of familiar faces. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(7), 1438-1450.

Lavie, N. (1995). Perceptual load as a necessary condition for selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 21(3), 451-468.

Lavie, N., Beck, D. M., & Konstantinou, N. (2014). Blinded by the load:attention, awareness and the role of perceptual load. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 369(1641), 20130205.

Lavie, N., Ro, T., & Russell, C. (2003). The role of perceptual load in processing distractor faces. Psychological Science, 14(5), 510-515.

Le Gal, P. M., & Bruce, V. (2002). Evaluating the independence of sex and expression in judgments of faces. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 64(2), 230-243.

Lewin, C., & Herlitz, A. (2002). Sex differences in face recognition-Women's faces make the difference. Brain and Cognition, 50(1), 121-128.

Lovén, J., Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2011). Women's own-gender bias in face recognition memory. The role of attention at encoding. Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 333-340.

Lovén, J., Svärd, J., Ebner, N. C., Herlitz, A., & Fischer, H. (2014). Face gender modulates women's brain activity during face encoding. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(7), 1000-1005.

Macrae, C. N., Bodenhausen, G. V., Milne, A. B., & Ford, R. L. (1997). On regulation of recollection:The intentional forgetting of stereotypical memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(4), 709-719.

Macrae, C. N., Quinn, K. A., Mason, M. F., & Quadflieg, S. (2005). Understanding others:The face and person construal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 686-695.

Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., & Giard, M.-H. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of age and gender perception on human faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(6), 900-910.

Murphy, G., Groeger, J. A., & Greene, C. M. (2016). Twenty years of load theory-Where are we now, and where should we go next? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(5), 1316-1340.

Neumann, M. F., Mohamed, T. N., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2011). Face and object encoding under perceptual load:ERP evidence. NeuroImage, 54(4), 3021-3027.

Neumann, M. F., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2009). N250r ERP repetition effects from distractor faces when attending to another face under load:Evidence for a face attention resource. Brain Research, 1270, 64-77.

O'Callaghan, C., Kveraga, K., Shine, J. M., Adams, R. B., & Bar, M. (2016). Convergent evidence for top-down effects from the "predictive brain". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e254.

Olk, B., & Garay-Vado, A. M. (2011). Attention to faces:Effects of face inversion. Vision Research, 51(14), 1659-1666.

Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science, 309(5735), 785-787.

Palermo, R., & Rhodes, G. (2007). Are you always on my mind? A review of how face perception and attention interact. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 75-92.

Park, G., van Bavel, J. J., Hill, L. K., Williams, D. P., & Thayer, J. F. (2016). Social groups prioritize selective attention to faces:How social identity shapes distractor interference. PLoS One, 11(10), e0164909.

Peng, X. H., Luo, Y, J., Wei, J. H., & Wang, G. F. (2002). Cognitive model and electrophysiologic evidence of face recognition. Advances in Psychological Science, 10(3), 241-247.

[彭小虎, 罗跃嘉, 魏景汉, 王国锋. (2002). 面孔识别的认知模型与电生理学证据. 心理科学进展, 10(3), 241-247.]

Rossion, B. (2008). Picture-plane inversion leads to qualitative changes of face perception. Acta Psychologica, 128(2), 274-289.

Schiltz, C., & Rossion, B. (2006). Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex. NeuroImage, 32(3), 1385-1394.

Sporer, S. L. (2001). Recognizing faces of other ethnic groups:An integration of theories. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 36-97.

Sreenivasan, K. K., Goldstein, J. M., Lustig, A. G., Rivas, L. R., & Jha, A. P. (2009). Attention to faces modulates early face processing during low but not high face discriminability. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 837-846.

Sun, H.-M., & Balas, B. (2012). The influence of flankers on race categorization of faces. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 74(8), 1654-1667.

Tahmasebi, A. M., Artiges, E., Banaschewski, T., Barker, G. J., Bruehl, R., Büchel, C.,... Paus, T. (2012). Creating probabilistic maps of the face network in the adolescent brain:A multicentre functional MRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 33(4), 938-957.

Valuch, C., Pflüger, L. S., Wallner, B., Laeng, B., & Ansorge, U. (2015). Using eye tracking to test for individual differences in attention to attractive faces. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 42.

Wang, H. L., Ip, C., Fu, S. M., & Sun, P. (2017). Different underlying mechanisms for face emotion and gender processing during feature-selective attention:Evidence from event-related potential studies. Neuropsychologia, 99, 306-313.

Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling racial prejudice social-cognitive goals affect amygdala and stereotype activation. Psychological Science, 16(1), 56-63.

Wiese, H., Kloth, N., Güllmar, D., Reichenbach, J. R., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2012). Perceiving age and gender in unfamiliar faces:An fMRI study on face categorization. Brain and Cognition, 78(2), 163-168.

Wirth, B. E., Fisher, K., Towler, J., & Eimer, M. (2015). Facial misidentifications arise from the erroneous activation of visual face memory. Neuropsychologia, 77, 387-399.

Yang, P., Wang, M., Jin, Z. L., & Li, L. (2015). Visual short-term memory load modulates the early attention and perception of task-irrelevant emotional faces. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 490.

Yokoyama, T., Noguchi, Y., Tachibana, R., Mukaida, S., & Kita, S. (2014). A critical role of holistic processing in face gender perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 477.

Young, S. G., Brown, C. M., & Ambady, N. (2012). Priming a natural or human-made environment directs attention to context-congruent threatening stimuli. Cognition & Emotion, 26(5), 927-933.