doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.00703

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2018/50:7 PP.703-714

Landmark attraction effect and landmark repulsion effect on representational momentum in airplane movement scene


Abstract:
The term representational momentum has been used to describe a forward displacement of the final position of a moving target and also as an explanatory mechanism for that forward displacement. Landmark attraction effect refers to the remembered orientation of a target shifted towards the orientation of a surrounding context object. Previous studies on moving targets and landmarks have been limited to the effects of physical properties, such as the size of the stimulus, or the speed of movement, on the displacement of another object. The present study focused on the effects of the situational significance, the relative relationship between the moving target and the relevant landmark, the movement directions of the target and the presentation time of the landmarks on the judgment of the moving target position.
The inducing movement paradigm was adopted to investigate the representational momentum effect in the flight scene, in which the airplane target and the landmarks were designed as simple line-drawing images. The stationary airstrip and the mountain were classified as safe landmarks and dangerous landmarks, respectively, forming a causal relationship with the aircraft target, which together constituted a specific aircraft movement scene. Four experiments were designed to compare the effects of different landmarks. Experiments 1a and 1b focused on the relationship between the aircraft and the safe landmark or dangerous landmark and the movement direction of the aircraft on the representational momentum, while the landmark remained visible in the induction duration. Experiments 2a and 2b addressed the same variable relationship as in Experiments 1a and 1b except that the landmark was shown during the retention interval.
The findings of the four experiments were as follows. First, significant forward distortions were observed under all conditions with the exception of leftward-toward the dangerous landmark motion, indicating that the representational momentum effect was universal in the flight scene. Second, forward displacement was influenced by target approaches and was larger when targets moved toward the safe landmark than when moving away from the same landmark. In addition, it was also larger when targets moved away from the dangerous landmark than when they moved toward it. The representational momentum of the approaching safe landmark was larger than that of the dangerous landmark, and the momentum of moving away from the safe landmark was smaller than that of the dangerous landmark. In this way, the safe landmark showed landmark an attraction effect, and the dangerous landmark showed a repulsion effect. Third, the direction of movement of the aircraft had no significant effect on the representational momentum, and the representational momentum increased when the safe landmark or the dangerous landmark was shown during the retention interval, demonstrating the effect of the scenario on the directional effect and attention effect of the momentum.
In conclusion, the results of this study suggested that the landmark effect of the representational momentum was influenced by the relevant features of the landmark, which was measured by the causality and the situational significance between the target and the landmark. The memory of the location of the moving object will shift along the direction of the object's movement, indicating that the momentum effect is difficult to eliminate. However, when there are other related objects (landmarks) in the scene, the nature and the importance of the object has an impact on people's judgment.

Key words:representational momentum,landmark attraction effect,landmark repulsion effect,scene,forward displacement

ReleaseDate:2018-08-06 13:38:59



Blätter, C., Ferrari, V., Didierjean, A., & Marmèche, E. (2011). Representational momentum in aviation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 37(5), 1569-1577.

Blätter, C., Ferrari, V., Didierjean, A., & Marmèche, E. (2012). Role of expertise and action in motion extrapolation from real road scenes. Visual Cognition, 20(8), 988-1001.

Courtney, J. R., & Hubbard, T. L. (2008). Spatial memory and explicit knowledge:An effect of instruction on representational momentum. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 1778-1784.

Didierjean, A., Ferrari, V., & Blättler, C. (2014). Role of knowledge in motion extrapolation:The relevance of an approach contrasting experts and novices. In B. H. Ross (Ed), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 215-235, Vol. 61). Burlington:Academic Press.

Dong, R. (2015). Facing orientation effect on representational momentum. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(2), 190-202.[董蕊. (2015). 表征动量的朝向效应. 心理学报, 47(2), 190-202.]

Finke, R. A., & Freyd, J. J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 780-794.

Freyd, J. J. (1983). The mental representation of movement when static stimuli are viewed. Perception & Psychophysics, 33, 575-581.

Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126-132.

Freyd, J. J., & Jones, K. T. (1994). Representational momentum for a spiral path. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 968-976.

Freyd, J. J., & Miller, G. F. (1992, November). Creature motion. Paper presented at the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Psychonomic Society, St. Louis.

Freyd, J. J., & Pantzer, T. M. (1995). Static patterns moving in the mind. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward, & R. A. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 181-204). Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.

Halpern, A. R., & Kelly, M. H. (1993). Memory biases in left versus right implied motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 471-484.

Hayes, A. E., & Freyd, J. J. (2002). Representational momentum when attention is divided. Visual Cognition, 9, 8-27.

Hubbard, T. L. (1990). Cognitive representation of linear motion:Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Memory and Cognition, 18, 299-309.

Hubbard, T. L. (1993). The effect of context on visual representational momentum. Memory and Cognition, 21, 103-114.

Hubbard, T. L. (1996). Representational momentum, centripetal force, and curvilinear impetus. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1049-1060.

Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory:A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 822-851.

Hubbard, T. L. (2008). Representational momentum contributes to motion induced mislocalization of stationary objects. Visual Cognition, 16(1), 44-67.

Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Approaches to representational momentum:Theories and models. In R. Nijhawan & B. Khurana (Eds.), Space and time in perception and action (pp. 338-365). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Hubbard, T. L. (2013a). Phenomenal causality Ⅰ:Varieties and variables. Axiomathes, 23(1), 1-42.

Hubbard, T. L. (2013b). Launching, entraining, and representational momentum:Evidence consistent with an impetus heuristic in perception of causality. Axiomathes, 23(4), 633-643.

Hubbard, T. L. (2014). Forms of momentum across space:Representational, operational, and attentional. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 21(6), 1371-1403.

Hubbard, T. L. (2017). Toward a general theory of momentum-like effects. Behavioural Processes, 141, 50-56.

Hubbard, T. L., & Bharucha, J. J. (1988). Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception and Psychophysics, 44(3), 211-221.

Hubbard, T. L., Blessum, J. A., & Ruppel, S. E. (2001). Representational momentum and Michotte's "Launching Effect" paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 294-301.

Hubbard, T. L., & Courtney, J. R. (2010). Cross-modal influences on representational momentum and representational gravity. Perception, 39, 851-862.

Hubbard, T. L., Kumar, A. M., & Carp, C. L. (2009). Effects of spatial cueing on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 666-677.

Hubbard, T. L., & Ruppel, S. E. (1999). Representational momentum and the landmark attraction effect. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(3), 242-256.

Hubbard, T. L., & Ruppel, S. E. (2000). Spatial memory averaging, the landmark attraction effect, and representational gravity. Psychological Research, 64(1), 41-55.

Hubbard, T. L., & Ruppel, S. E. (2017). Perceived causality, force, and resistance in the absence of launching. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24, 591-596.

Johnston, H. M., & Jones, M. R. (2006). Higher order pattern structure influences auditory representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 32, 2-17.

Joordens, S., Spalek, T. M., Razmy, S., & van Duijn, M. (2004). A clockwork orange:Compensation opposing momentum in memory for location. Memory and Cognition, 32(1), 39-50.

Kerzel, D. (2002). Attention shifts and memory averaging. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 425-443.

Kerzel, D. (2003). Attention maintains mental extrapolation of target position:Irrelevant distractors eliminate forward displacement after implied motion. Cognition, 88, 109-131.

Liu, H. Q. (2014). The influence of target motion on the momentum of representation (Unpublished doctorial dissertation). Capital Normal University.[刘浩强. (2014). 目标运动归因对表征动量的影响 (博士学位论文). 首都师范大学.]

Munger, M., & Owens, T. R. (2004). Representational momentum and the flash-lag effect. Visual Cognition, 11(1), 81-103.

Nagai, M., & Yagi, A. (2001). The pointedness effect on representational momentum. Memory and Cognition, 29(1), 91-99.

Rao, H. Y., Han, S. H., Jiang, Y., Xue, Y. P., Gu, H., Cui, Y., & Gao, D. G. (2004). Engagement of the prefrontal cortex in representational momentum:An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23(1), 98-103.

Reed, C. L., & Vinson, N. G. (1996). Conceptual effects on representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 22, 839-850.

Thornton, L., & Hayes, A. (2004). Anticipating action in complex scenes. Visual Cognition, 11, 341-370.

Whitney, D., & Cavanagh, P. (2002). Surrounding motion affects the perceived locations of moving stimuli. Visual Cognition, 9, 139-152.

Zhai, K., & Zhang, Z. J. (2012). The effect of spatial-temporal cue on representational momentum. Journal of Psychological Science, 35(6), 1309-1314.[翟坤, 张志杰. (2012). 线索呈现的时空特性对表征动量的影响. 心理科学, 35(6), 1309-1314.]

Zhai, K., & Zhang, Z. J. (2013). The effects of cues on representational momentum. Journal of Psychological Science, 36(1), 51-56. [翟坤, 张志杰. (2013). 线索提示对表征动量的影响. 心理科学, 36(1), 51-56.]