doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00055

Acta Psychologica Sinica (心理学报) 2020/52:1 PP.55-65

The pursuit of fame at the expense of profit: The influence of power motive and social presence on prosocial behavior


Abstract:
Power motive often aims at status and superiority and has been associated with antisocial decision-making, dehumanization of others, infidelity, and aggressive behaviors. In light of such findings, it is not surprising that the power motive has acquired a bad reputation. However, there is also a benevolent, prosocial side to power that has not received equal attention. From the beginning, researchers have emphasized the dual nature of power motive:people realize their power motive in either an antisocial or a prosocial direction. The canonical definition of power motive focuses on having an impact on others, which is not synonymous with being dominant. This is an important distinction, because the stereotypical picture associated with power is a kind of in-your-face aggressive and domineering behavior. Yet among many mammalian species, particularly primates, this type of behavior is rarely a sustainable strategy for attaining and maintaining dominance, and it is not what typically characterizes individuals high in power motive. Although such individuals can be aggressive, irresponsible, and uncooperative, they have more often been found to be clever and intelligent in their quest for impactful experiences.
Research has shown that social presence (e.g., a subtle cue of being watched) has a significant influence on individuals' behavior in social dilemmas. Specifically, it has been observed that individuals' tendency to engage in prosocial behavior increases when acting under conditions of a social presence. With respect to social presence, reputation has been discussed as a critical factor determining individuals' tendency to contribute to a public good and to behave prosocially. The relevant argument holds that individuals are willing to invest private resources under conditions where they can expect to build a positive reputation that may be beneficial in (future) social interactions involving indirect reciprocity. For example, research has demonstrated the status benefits of selfless behavior. Individuals pursue status by enhancing the apparent value they provide to their group and compete for status not by bullying and intimidating others, but by behaving in ways that suggest high levels of competence, generosity, and commitment to the group. This seemingly selfless behavior leads to them being perceived as more generous in their groups and, in turn, leads to a higher status and a good reputation. Therefore, individuals who sought reputation and status attained them by acting strategically prosocially.
The present work builds on previous research on social presence and reputation and addresses the question of whether the effect of power motive on prosocial behavior is dependent on social presence. In essence, the current work put the assumption to the test that, under conditions where a subtle cue of being watched (study 1) or public situation (study 2) render reputational concerns salient, individuals are more likely to act in fairness (study 1) or cooperation (study 2) if they have a higher level of power motive. In contrast, under anonymous conditions, individuals' power motive should not be related to fair and cooperative behaviors. The results confirmed our hypothesis that under conditions where a subtle cue of being watched or in a public situation, high power motive individuals, relative to low power motive participants, allocated more money to interactive partners in the ultimatum game and provided higher provision levels of public goods in the public good game. On the contrary, under anonymous conditions, no significant relationship was found between individuals' power motive and fair and cooperative behaviors.
The results suggest that people with high power motive also exhibit prosocial behaviors in consideration of strategies of reputation and status. The present work demonstrates that power motive can play a critical role in social dilemma situations. Moreover, the findings emphasize that one must take the specificity of a situation into account (particularly, whether social presence as a situational factor influences individuals' decisions) in order to explain individuals' behavior in dilemma situations.

Key words:power motive,social presence,reputation,fairness,cooperation,prosocial behavior

ReleaseDate:2019-12-28 15:34:12



Acton, J. E. E. D. A., & Himmelfarb, G. (1948). Essays on freedom and power. Boston, MA:Beacon Press.

Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009). The pursuit of status in social groups. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 295-298.

Aydinli, A., Bender, M., Chasiotis, A., Cemalcilar, Z., & van de Vijver, F. (2014). When does self-reported prosocial motivation predict helping? The moderating role of implicit prosocial motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 38(5), 645-658.

Barclay, P. (2013). Strategies for cooperation in biological markets, especially for humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(3), 164-175.

Bereczkei, T., Birkas, B., & Kerekes, Z. (2010). The presence of others, prosocial traits, machiavellianism. Social Psychology, 41(4), 238-245.

Bergstrom, C. T., & Lachmann, M. (2001). Alarm calls as costly signals of antipredator vigilance:The watchful babbler game. Animal Behaviour, 61(3), 535-543.

Boehm, C., & Flack, J. C. (2010). The emergence of simple and complex power structures through social niche construction. In A. Guinote & T. K. Vescio (Eds.), The social psychology of power (pp. 46-86). New York, NY, US:Guilford Press.

Boksem, M. A. S., Mehta, P. H., van den Bergh, B., van Son, V., Trautmann, S. T., Roelofs, K.,... Sanfey, A. G. (2013). Testosterone inhibits trust but promotes reciprocity. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2306-2314.

Carbon, C. C., & Hesslinger, V. M. (2011). Bateson et al.'s (2006) cues-of-being-watched paradigm revisited. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 70(4), 203-210.

Chasiotis, A., Hofer, J., & Campos, D. (2006). When does liking children lead to parenthood? Younger siblings, implicit prosocial power motivation, and explicit love for children predict parenthood across cultures. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4(2), 95-123.

Dabbs, J. M., Carr, T. S., Frady, R. L., & Riad, J. K. (1995). Testosterone, crime, and misbehavior among 692 male prison inmates. Personality and Individual Differences, 18(5), 627-633.

de Waal, F. B. (1998). Chimpanzee politics:Power and sex among apes. Baltimore, MD:Johns Hopkins University Press.

Ditlmann, R. K., Purdie-Vaughns, V., Dovidio, J. F., & Naft, M. J. (2017). The implicit power motive in intergroup dialogues about the history of slavery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(1), 116-135.

Donhauser, P. W., Rösch, A. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2015). The implicit need for power predicts recognition speed for dynamic changes in facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 39(5), 714-721.

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Sprinrad, T. L. (1998). Prosocial development. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701-778). New York:Wiley.

Eisenegger, C., Naef, M., Snozzi, R., Heinrichs, M., & Fehr, E. (2010). Prejudice and truth about the effect of testosterone on human bargaining behaviour, Nature, 463(7279), 356-359.

Ekström, M. (2012). Do watching eyes affect charitable giving? Evidence from a field experiment. Experimental Economics, 15(3), 530-546.

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3:A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.

Fodor, E. M., Wick, D. P., & Conroy, N. E. (2012). Power motivation as an influence on reaction to an imagined feminist dating partner. Motivation and Emotion, 36(3), 301-310.

Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144(4), 517-546.

Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., & van den Bergh, B. (2010). Going green to be seen:Status, reputation, and conspicuous conservation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(3), 392-404.

Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 3(4), 367-388.

Hardy, C. L., & van Vugt, M. (2006). Nice guys finish first:The competitive altruism hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 1402-1413.

Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H.,... Henrich, N. S. (2005). "Economic man" in cross-cultural perspective:Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(6), 795-815.

Hoffmann, R., & Tee, J. Y. (2006). Adolescent-adult interactions and culture in the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 27(1), 98-116.

Inoue, Y., Takahashi, T., Burriss, R. P., Arai, S., Hasegawa, T., Yamagishi, T., & Kiyonari, T. (2017). Testosterone promotes either dominance or submissiveness in the Ultimatum Game depending on players' social rank. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 5335.

Jiao, C., Zhang, M. Q., Wu, L., & Ji, W. (2010). The revised short version of multi-motive grid in mainland China. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 8(1), 49-53.

[焦璨, 张敏强, 吴利, 纪薇. (2010). 多元动机网格测验(MMG-S)中文版的修订报告. 心理与行为研究, 8(1), 49-53.]

Keller, J., & Pfattheicher, S. (2011). Vigilant self-regulation, cues of being watched and cooperativeness. European Journal of Personality, 25(5), 363-372.

Kopányi-Peuker, A., Offerman, T., & Sloof, R. (2017). Fostering cooperation through the enhancement of own vulnerability. Games and Economic Behavior, 101, 273-290.

Kurzban, R., DeScioli, P., & O'Brien, E. (2007). Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(2), 75-84.

Lammers, J., & Stapel, D. A. (2011). Power increases dehumanization. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 14(1), 113-126.

Lammers, J., Stoker, J. I., Jordan, J., Pollmann, M. M. H., & Stapel, D. A. (2011). Power increases infidelity among men and women. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1191-1197.

Lien, J. W., & Zheng, J. (2019). Self-commitment for cooperation. Working Paper.

Lien, J. W., Zheng, J., & Zhuo, Y. (2018). The cooperative consequences of contests. Working Paper.

Magee, J. C., & Langner, C. A. (2008). How personalized and socialized power motivation facilitate antisocial and prosocial decision-making. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(6), 1547-1559.

Maner, J. K., & Mead, N. L. (2010). The essential tension between leadership and power:When leaders sacrifice group goals for the sake of self-interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3), 482-497.

Manesi, Z., van Lange, P. A. M., & Pollet, T. V. (2015). Butterfly eyespots:Their potential influence on aesthetic preferences and conservation attitudes. PloS One, 10(11), e0141433.

McClelland, D. C. (1970). The two faces of power. Journal of International Affairs, 24(1), 29-47.

Pfattheicher, S., & Keller, J. (2015). The watching eyes phenomenon:The role of a sense of being seen and public self-awareness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 45(5), 560-566.

Ridgeway, C., & Diekema, D. (1989). Dominance and collective hierarchy formation in male and female task groups. American Sociological Review, 54(1), 79-93.

Russell, B. (1938). Power:A new social analysis. London:George Allen & Unwin Ltd.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2002). Inhibited power motivation and persuasive communication:A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality, 70(4), 553-582.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Hale, J. A. (2007). Implicit motives modulate attentional orienting to facial expressions of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 31(1), 13-24.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Köllner, M. G. (in press). Implicit motives. In O. P. John & R. W. Robins (Eds.), Handbook of personality:Theory and research (4 ed.). New York:Guilford.

Schultheiss, O. C., Wirth, M. M., Waugh, C. E., Stanton, S. J., Meier, E. A., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2008). Exploring the motivational brain:Effects of implicit power motivation on brain activation in response to facial expressions of emotion. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(4), 333-343.

Sellers, J. G., Mehl, M. R., & Josephs, R. A. (2007). Hormones and personality:Testosterone as a marker of individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 126-138.

Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H. D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M. (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives all at once:The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG). Journal of Personality Assessment, 74(1), 126-145.

Stanton, S. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2009). The hormonal correlates of implicit power motivation. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(5), 942-949.

van Honk, J., Montoya, E. R., Bos, P. A., van Vugt, M., & Terburg, D. (2012). New evidence on testosterone and cooperation. Nature, 485(7399), E4-E5.

Vermeersch, H., T'sjoen, G., Kaufman, J. M., Vincke, J., & van Houtte, M. (2010). Gender ideology, same-sex peer group affiliation and the relationship between testosterone and dominance in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Biosocial Science, 42(4), 463-475.

Vongas, J. G., & Al Hajj, R. (2017). The effects of competition and implicit power motive on men's testosterone, emotion recognition, and aggression. Hormones and Behavior, 92, 57-71.

Wang, J. F., Liu, L., Yang, Q. W., Zhang, J. P., & Yan, J. H. (2017). The implicit need for power predicts recognition memory for anger faces:An electrophysiological study. Personality and Individual Differences, 109, 207-214.

Wang, J. F., Liu, L., & Yan, J. H. (2014). Implicit power motive effects on the ERP processing of emotional intensity in anger faces. Journal of Research in Personality, 50, 90-97.

Wang, J. F., Liu, L., & Zheng, Y. (2011). Effects of implicit power motive on the processing of anger faces:An event-related potential study. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(5), 441-447.

Weber, J. M., Kopelman, S., & Messick, D. M. (2004). A conceptual review of decision making in social dilemmas:Applying a logic of appropriateness. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(3), 281-307.

Wedekind, C., & Milinski, M. (2000). Cooperation through image scoring in humans. Science, 288(5467), 850-852.

Wibral, M., Dohmen, T., Klingmüller, D., Weber, B., & Falk, A. (2012). Testosterone administration reduces lying in men. PloS One, 7(10), e46774.

Willer, R. (2009). Groups reward individual sacrifice:The status solution to the collective action problem. American Sociological Review, 74(1), 23-43.

Winter, D. G. (1973). The power motive. New York:Free Press.

Xin, Z. Q., Liu, Y. H., Yang, Z. X., & Zhang, H. C. (2016). Effects of minimal social cues on trust in the investment game. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(3), 235-243.

Zurbriggen, E. L. (2000). Social motives and cognitive power-sex associations:Predictors of aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 559-581.