doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.01337

Advances in Psychological Science (心理科学进展) 2017/25:8 PP.1337-1348

Specific influence of Open Monitoring Meditation


Abstract:
Open Monitoring (OM) Meditation has received many researchers' attention in recent years. Compared with other meditation, especially with Focus Attention (FA) Meditation, OM has been shown more effective influence on some cognitive processes (such as pain and creative thinking) and anxiety. While, there was not consistent result in attention currently. Several other studies have explored neural mechanisms of OM with electroencephalograph and fMRI, especially in the default mode network, although inconsistent result was found. Finally, OM approach adopted in these studies showed obvious differences, such as whether or not starting from breath and maintaining attention. These results need to be further clarified in future studies, adopting more standardized approach.

Key words:meditation,open monitoring,default mode network

ReleaseDate:2017-08-31 10:17:32



刘兴华, 徐钧, 张琴, 吴燕霞. (2016). "此刻觉察"正念训练的定义、操作及可行性. 中国健康心理学杂志, 24, 1224-1229.

齐臻臻, 刘兴华. (2016). 此刻觉察冥想方案的有效性:来自冷压任务的证据. 中国临床心理学杂志, 24, 566-571.

Ainsworth, B., Eddershaw, R., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., & Garner, M. (2013). The effect of focused attention and open monitoring meditation on attention network function in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 210, 1226-1231.

Ainsworth, B., Marshall, J. E., Meron, D., Baldwin, D. S., Chadwick, P., Munafò, M. R., & Garner, M. (2015). Evaluating psychological interventions in a novel experimental human model of anxiety. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 63, 117-122.

Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007). Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14, 449-463.

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., & Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain's default network. Neuron, 65, 550-562.

Baas, M., Nevicka, B., & Ten Velden, F. S. (2014). Specific mindfulness skills differentially predict creative performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 1092-1106.

Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 20254-20259.

Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215-222.

Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747-749.

Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 449-464.

Colle, K. F. F., Vincent, A., Cha, S. S., Loehrer, L. L., Bauer, B. A., & Wahner-Roedler, D. L. (2010). Measurement of quality of life and participant experience with the mindfulness-based stress reduction program. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 16, 36-40.

Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012). Meditate to create:The impact of focused-attention and open-monitoring training on convergent and divergent thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 116.

Colzato, L. S., Sellaro, R., Samara, I., Baas, M., & Hommel, B. (2015). Meditation-induced states predict attentional control over time. Consciousness and Cognition, 37, 57-62.

Colzato, L. S., Sellaro, R., Samara, I., & Hommel, B. (2015). Meditation-induced cognitive-control states regulate response-conflict adaptation:Evidence from trial-to-trial adjustments in the Simon task. Consciousness and Cognition, 35, 110-114.

Colzato, L. S., Szapora, A., Lippelt, D., & Hommel, B. (2014). Prior meditation practice modulates performance and strategy use in convergent-and divergent-thinking problems. Mindfulness, 8, 10-16.

Colzato, L. S., van der Wel, P., Sellaro, R., & Hommel, B. (2016). A single bout of meditation biases cognitive control but not attentional focusing:Evidence from the global-local task. Consciousness and Cognition, 39, 1-7.

Cusens, B., Duggan, G. B., Thorne, K., & Burch, V. (2010). Evaluation of the breathworks mindfulness-based pain management programme:Effects on well-being and multiple measures of mindfulness. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 17, 63-78.

du Boisgueheneuc, F., Levy, R., Volle, E., Seassau, M., Duffau, H., Kinkingnehun, S.,... Dubois, B. (2006). Functions of the left superior frontal gyrus in humans:A lesion study. Brain, 129, 3315-3328.

Elliott, R., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Dissociable functions in the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex:evidence from human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 308-317.

Fransson, P., & Marrelec, G. (2008). The precuneus/posterior cingulate cortex plays a pivotal role in the default mode network:Evidence from a partial correlation network analysis. Neuroimage, 42, 1178-1184.

Goldberg, I. I., Harel, M., & Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self:Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50, 329-339.

Hinterberger, T., Schmidt, S., Kamei, T., & Walach, H. (2014). Decreased electrophysiological activity represents the conscious state of emptiness in meditation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 99.

Huang, H. Y., & Lo, P. C. (2009). EEG dynamics of experienced Zen meditation practitioners probed by complexity index and spectral measure. Journal of Medical Engineering & Technology, 33, 314-321.

Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living:Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York, NY:Delacourt Press.

Kaiser, J., & Lutzenberger, W. (2005). Human gamma-band activity:A window to cognitive processing. Neuroreport, 16, 207-211.

Kringelbach, M. L., & Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex:Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology, 72, 341-372.

Lippelt, D. P., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2014). Focused attention, open monitoring and loving kindness meditation:Effects on attention, conflict monitoring, and creativity-A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1083.

Lutz, A., Jha, A. P., Dunne, J. D., & Saron, C. D. (2015). Investigating the phenomenological matrix of mindfulness-related practices from a neurocognitive perspective. American Psychologist, 70, 632-658.

Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 163-169.

Manna, A., Raffone, A., Perrucci, M. G., Nardo, D., Ferretti, A., Tartaro, A.,... Romani, G. L. (2010). Neural correlates of focused attention and cognitive monitoring in meditation. Brain Research Bulletin, 82, 46-56.

Marzetti, L., Lanzo, C. D., Zappasodi, F., Chella, F., Raffone, A., & Pizzella, V. (2014). Magnetoencephalographic alpha band connectivity reveals differential default mode network interactions during focused attention and open monitoring meditation. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 832.

Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds:The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393-395.

Ospina, M. B., Bond, T. K., Karkhaneh, M., Tjosvold, L., Vandermeer, B., Liang, Y.,... Klassen, T. P. (2007). Meditation practices for health:State of the research (p. 10). Evidence Report/Technology Assessment No. 155. Rockville:AHRQ.

Perlman, D. M., Salomons, T. V., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz, A. (2010). Differential effects on pain intensity and unpleasantness of two meditation practices. Emotion, 10, 65-71.

Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 676-682.

Razumnikova, O. M. (2007). Creativity related cortex activity in the remote associates task. Brain Research Bulletin, 73, 96-102.

Reiner, K., Tibi, L., & Lipsitz, J. D. (2013). Do mindfulness-based interventions reduce pain intensity? A critical review of the literature. Pain Medicine, 14, 230-242.

Sedlmeier, P., Eberth, J., Schwarz, M., Zimmermann, D., Haarig, F., Jaeger, S., & Kunze, S. (2012). The psychological effects of meditation:A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 1139-1171.

Shear, J. (2006). The experience of meditation:Experts introduce the major traditions (p. 286). St. Paul, MN:Paragon House.

Tanaka, G. K., Maslahati, T., Gongora, M., Bittencourt, J., Lopez, L. C. S., Demarzo, M. M. P.,... Velasques, B. (2015). Effortless attention as a biomarker for experienced mindfulness practitioners. PLoS One, 10, e0138561.

Travis, F., & Shear, J. (2010). Focused attention, open monitoring and automatic self-transcending:Categories to organize meditations from Vedic, Buddhist and Chinese traditions. Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 1110-1118.

van Vugt, M. K., & Slagter, H. A. (2014). Control over experience? magnitude of the attentional blink depends on meditative state. Consciousness and Cognition, 23, 32-39.

Wilson, B. M., Mickes, L., Stolarz-Fantino, S., Evrard, M., & Fantino, E. (2015). Increased false-memory susceptibility after mindfulness meditation. Psychological Science, 26, 1567-1573.