doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2017.01705

Advances in Psychological Science (心理科学进展) 2017/25:10 PP.1705-1712

The relationship between infants'pointing gestures and language learning


Abstract:
A pointing gesture is the simultaneous extension of the arm and index finger to something in space, such as an object, a person, a location, or an event. Pointing emerges around 12 months of age, and is positively associated with infants' language learning; however, it is not clear why such a correlation exists. In this paper, we consider two processes that could influence the relationship between pointing and language development:(1) pointing may influence learning by eliciting contingent linguistic input from the social environment; (2) pointing may create an effective learning state in infants. Future studies are needed to examine how the dynamic interaction of these two processes facilitate language learning, and to explore the use of pointing as a diagnostic or intervention tool for children who are at risk for language delays.

Key words:pointing gesture,language learning,joint attention,language delay,infants

ReleaseDate:2017-11-17 09:48:02



Begus, K., Gliga, T., & Southgate, V. (2014). Infants learn what they want to learn:Responding to infant pointing leads to superior learning. PLoS One, 9, e108817.

Begus, K., & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15, 611-617.

Blake, J., Vitale, G., Osborne, P., & Olshansky, E. (2005). A cross-cultural comparison of communicative gestures in human infants during the transition to language. Gesture, 5, 201-217.

Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age:A longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language, 35, 207-220.

Butterworth, G., & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 14, 219-231.

Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(4), 1-143.

Cartmill, E. A., Hunsicker, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Pointing and naming are not redundant:Children use gesture to modify nouns before they modify nouns in speech. Developmental Psychology, 50, 1660-1666.

Cochet, H., & Byrne, R. W. (2016). Communication in the second and third year of life:Relationships between nonverbal social skills and language. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 189-198.

Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development:A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30, 352-366.

Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K.Y., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing makes memories that last. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 465-475.

Delgado, B., Gómez, J. C., & Sarriá, E. (2011). Pointing gestures as a cognitive tool in young children:Experimental evidence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 299-312.

Dice, J. L., & Dove, M. K. (2011). A Piagetian approach to infant referential behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 34, 481-486.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language-and creating language. Child Development, 78, 741-745.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Widening the lens:What the manual modality reveals about language, learning and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 369, 20130295.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015a). Gesture as a window onto communicative abilities:Implications for diagnosis and intervention. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 22, 50-60.

Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015b). Studying the mechanisms of language learning by varying the learning environment and the learner. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 899-911.

Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10, 778-785.

Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math:Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12, 516-522.

Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J., Briesch, J., & Syal, S. (2010). Learning while babbling:Prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. Infancy, 15, 362-391.

Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16, 367-371.

Kishimoto, T., Shizawa, Y., Yasuda, J., Hinobayashi, T., & Minami, T. (2007). Do pointing gestures by infants provoke comments from adults? Infant Behavior and Development, 30, 562-567.

Kovács, Á. M., Tauzin, T., Téglás, E., Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2014). Pointing as epistemic request:12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy, 19, 543-557.

LeBarton, E. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Raudenbush, S. (2015). Experimentally induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16, 199-220.

Lee, Y., & Lee, H. (2015). Communicative gestures in toddlers with developmental language delay and their relations to language development. Communication Sciences & Disorders, 20, 255-265.

Liszkowski, U. (2014). Pointing. In P. Brooks & V. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language development (Vol. 15, pp. 471-473). Thousand Oaks, CA:SAGE Publications, Inc.

Lüke, C., Grimminger, A., Rohlfing, K. J., Liszkowski, U., & Ritterfeld, U. (2017). In infants' hands:Identification of preverbal infants at risk for primary language delay. Child Development, 88, 484-492.

Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Origins of the human pointing gesture:A training study. Developmental Science, 15, 817-829.

Meng, X. W., & Hashiya, K. (2014). Pointing behavior in infants reflects the communication partner's attentional and knowledge states:A possible case of spontaneous informing. PLoS One, 9, e107579.

Mumford, K. H., & Kita, S. (2016). At 10-12 months, pointing gesture handedness predicts the size of receptive vocabularies. Infancy, 21, 751-765.

Novack, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015). Learning from gesture:How our hands change our minds. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 405-412.

Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2013). Mothers respond differently to infants' gestural versus nongestural communicative bids. First Language, 33, 372-387.

Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2015). Mothers' labeling responses to infants' gestures predict vocabulary outcomes. Journal of Child Language, 42, 1289-1311.

Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009a). Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323, 951-953.

Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009b). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12, 182-187.

Salomo, D., & Liszkowski, U. (2013). Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child Development, 84, 1296-1307.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78, 705-722.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin, Trans., Rev. ed). Cambridge, MA:The MIT Press.

Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2014). Infants' prelinguistic communicative acts and maternal responses:Relations to linguistic development. First Language, 34, 72-90.

Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2015a). Caregivers provide more labeling responses to infants' pointing than to infants' object-directed vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 42, 538-561.

Wu, Z., & Gros-Louis, J. (2015b). Contingent labeling after infants' pointing helps infants learn words. In D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, & P. P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2685-2690). Austin, TX:Cognitive Science Society.

Yu, C., & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition, 125, 244-262.