doi:

DOI: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00614

Advances in Psychological Science (心理科学进展) 2018/26:4 PP.614-624

Influencing factors and thought of temporal order perception


Abstract:
Temporal order perception is the perception of the simultaneity, successiveness and order of events in a few milliseconds to hundreds of milliseconds. The exploration of the influencing factors of temporal order perception can be divided into two research orientations:bottom-up and top-down. The bottom-up study explored the effects of stimulus properties, the biological characteristics of the subjects, and measuring methods on temporal order perception. And the top-down study explored the effects of the causal beliefs, emotions and culture on temporal order perception. In the future, it is necessary to combine the behavioral and neuroscience techniques, explore the two processes of temporal order perception to examine whether they are separated or simultaneous, and reveal the brain area that controls timing processing.

Key words:temporal order perception,bottom-up processing,top-down processing

ReleaseDate:2018-05-04 05:07:48



邓铸. (2005). 眼动心理学的理论、技术及应用研究. 南京师大学报(社会科学版), (1), 90-95.

吴念阳, 徐凝婷, 张琰. (2007). 空间图式加工促进方向性时间表述的理解. 心理科学, 30(4), 853-856.

杨林霖, 张志杰, 顾艳艳, 周文杰. (2013). 空间-时间联合编码效应:来自手动和眼动证据. 心理科学, 36(6), 1347-1354.

袁祥勇, 陈有国, 毕翠华, 刘培朵, 黄希庭. (2013). 时序知觉:中枢计时与分布计时. 科学通报, 58(8-9), 2895-2905.

张锋, 黄希庭, 郭秀艳. (2009). 时序知觉重复启动效应的作用机制. 心理学报, 41(3), 233-241.

张锋, 苏丹, 陈有国. (2015). 时序知觉重复启动效应的ERP研究. 西南大学学报(社会科学版), 41(1), 118-122.

Anderson, A. K. (2005). Affective influences on the attentional dynamics supporting awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 134(2), 258-281.

Anderson, A. K., Christoff, K., Panitz, D., De Rosa, E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural correlates of the automatic processing of threat facial signals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(13), 5627-5633.

Aschersleben, G., & Müsseler, J. (1999). Dissociations in the timing of stationary and moving stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 25(6), 1709-1720.

Bachmann, T., Põder, E., & Luiga, I. (2004). Illusory reversal of temporal order:The bias to report a dimmer stimulus as the first. Vision Research, 44(3), 241-246.

Bao, Y., Szymaszek, A., Wang, X. Y., Oron, A., Pöppel, E., & Szelag, E. (2013). Temporal order perception of auditory stimuli is selectively modified by tonal and non-tonal language environments. Cognition, 129(3), 579-585.

Bechlivanidis, C., & Lagnado, D. A. (2013). Does the "why" tell us the "when"? Psychological Science, 24(8), 1563-1572.

Bechlivanidis, C., & Lagnado, D. A. (2016). Time reordered:Causal perception guides the interpretation of temporal order. Cognition, 146, 58-66.

Becker, S. I., Horstmann, G., & Remington, R. W. (2011). Perceptual grouping, not emotion, accounts for search asymmetries with schematic faces. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 37(6), 1739-1757.

Bertelson, P., & Aschersleben, G. (2003). Temporal ventriloquism:crossmodal interaction on the time dimension:1. Evidence from auditory-visual temporal order judgment. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50(1-2), 147-155.

Blagrove, E., & Watson, D. G. (2010). Visual marking and facial affect:Can an emotional face be ignored? Emotion, 10(2), 147-168.

Boenke, L. T., Deliano, M., & Ohl, F. W. (2009). Stimulus duration influences perceived simultaneity in audiovisual temporal-order judgment. Experimental Brain Research, 198(2-3), 233-244.

Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space:Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 2257-2273.

Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought?:Mandarin and English speakers' conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 1-22.

Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118(1), 123-129.

Buehner, M. J. (2012). Understanding the past, predicting the future:Causation, not intentional action, is the root of temporal binding. Psychological Science, 23(12), 1490-1497.

Buehner, M. J., & Humphreys, G. R. (2009). Causal binding of actions to their effects:Research article. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1221-1228.

Busey, T., Craig, J., Clark, C., & Humes, L. (2010). Age-related changes in visual temporal order judgment performance:Relation to sensory and cognitive capacities. Vision Research, 50(17), 1628-1640.

Chen, J. Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104(2), 427-436.

Chen, Y. C., Shore, D. I., Lewis, T. L., & Maurer, D. (2016). The development of the perception of audiovisual simultaneity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 17-33.

Craig, J. C., & Busey, T. A. (2003). The effect of motion on tactile and visual temporal order judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(1), 81-94.

Craig, J. C., Rhodes, R. P., Busey, T. A., Kewley-Port, D., & Humes, L. E. (2010). Aging and tactile temporal order. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(1), 226-235.

Crawford, L. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2002). Learning where to look for danger:Integrating affective and spatial information. Psychological Science, 13(5), 449-453.

De Boer-Schellekens, L., & Vroomen, J. (2014). Multisensory integration compensates loss of sensitivity of visual temporal order in the elderly. Experimental Brain Research, 232(1), 253-262.

Desantis, A., Roussel, C., & Waszak, F. (2011). On the influence of causal beliefs on the feeling of agency. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1211-1220.

Desantis, A., Waszak, F., Moutsopoulou, K., & Haggard, P. (2016). How action structures time:About the perceived temporal order of action and predicted outcomes. Cognition, 146, 100-109.

Eagleman, D. M. (2008). Human time perception and its illusions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 131-136.

Fecica, A. M., & Stolz, J. A. (2008). Facial affect and temporal order judgments:Emotions turn back the clock. Experimental Psychology, 55(1), 3-8.

Fernbach, P. M., Linson-Gentry, P., & Sloman, S. A. (2007). Causal beliefs influence the perception of temporal order. In Proceedings of the 29th annual cognitive science society (pp. 269-274). Cognitive Science Society.

Fink, M., Churan, J., & Wittmann, M. (2005). Assessment of auditory temporal-order thresholds-A comparison of different measurement procedures and the influences of age and gender. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 23(5-6), 281-296.

Fink, M., Ulbrich, P., Churan, J., & Wittmann, M. (2006). Stimulus-dependent processing of temporal order. Behavioural Processes, 71(2-3), 344-352.

Fitch, R. H., Brown, C. P., O'Connor, K., & Tallal, P. (1993). Functional lateralization for auditory temporal processing in male and female rats. Behavioral Neurosciences, 107, 844-850.

Flumini, A., & Santiago, J. (2013). Time (also) flies from left to right… if it is needed. In Proceedings of the 36th annual meeting of the cognitive science society (pp. 2315-2320). Cognitive Science Society.

Fuhrman, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Cross-cultural differences in mental representations of time:Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 1430-1451.

Fuhrman, O., Boroditsky, L., & Hall, J. (2007). Mental time-lines follow writing direction:Comparing English and Hebrew speakers. In Proceedings of the 29th annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 1007-1011). Cognitive Science Society.

Fujisaki, W., Shimojo, S., Kashino, M., & Nishida, S. (2004). Recalibration of audiovisual simultaneity. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 773-778.

Gallace, A., & Spence, C. (2005). Visual capture of apparent limb position influences tactile temporal order judgments. Neuroscience Letters, 379(1), 63-68.

Geffen, G., Rosa, V., & Luciano, M. (2000). Effects of preferred hand and sex on the perception of tactile simultaneity. Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology, 22(2), 219-231.

Haering, C., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Mine is earlier than yours:Causal beliefs influence the perceived time of action effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 393.

Hancock, P. A., & Rausch, R. (2010). The effects of sex, age, and interval duration on the perception of time. Acta Psychologica, 133(2), 170-179.

Heed, T., Backhaus, J., & Röder, B. (2012). Integration of hand and finger location in external spatial coordinates for tactile localization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 38(2), 386-401.

Hermosillo, R., Ritterband-Rosenbaum, A., & van Donkelaar, P. (2011). Predicting future sensorimotor states influences current temporal decision making. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(27), 10019-10022.

Hilkenmeier, F., Olivers, C. N. L., & Scharlau, I. (2012). Prior entry and temporal attention:Cueing affects order errors in RSVP. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 38(1), 180-190.

Hillyard, S. A., & Anllo-Vento, L. (1998). Event-related brain potentials in the study of visual selective attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 95(3), 781-787.

Hirsh, I. J., & Sherrick, C. E., Jr. (1961). Perceived order in different sense modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(5), 423-432.

Hodsoll, S., Viding, E., & Lavie, N. (2011). Attentional capture by irrelevant emotional distractor faces. Emotion, 11(2), 346-353.

Hopfinger, J. B., & West, V. M. (2006). Interactions between endogenous and exogenous attention on cortical visual processing. NeuroImage, 31(2), 774-789.

Huang, S.-L., Chang, Y.-C., & Chen, Y.-J. (2011). Task-irrelevant angry faces capture attention in visual search while modulated by resources. Emotion, 11(3), 544-552.

Humes, L. E., Kewley-Port, D., Fogerty, D., & Kinney, D. (2010). Measures of hearing threshold and temporal processing across the adult lifespan. Hearing Research, 264(1-2), 30-40.

Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., & Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal spatial representations of time:Evidence for the STEARC effect. Cortex, 44(4), 454-461.

Jaśkowski, P. (1993). Selective attention and temporal-order judgment. Perception, 22(6), 681-689.

Kanabus, M., Szelag, E., Rojek, E., & Pöppel, E. (2002). Temporal order judgement for auditory and visual stimuli. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 62(4), 263-270.

Kanai, K., Ikeda, K., & Tayama, T. (2007). The effect of exogenous spatial attention on auditory information processing. Psychological Research, 71(4), 418-426.

Keetels, M., & Vroomen, J. (2005). The role of spatial disparity and hemifields in audio-visual temporal order judgments. Experimental Brain Research, 167(4), 635-640.

Kołodziejczyk, I., & Szelsg, E. (2008). Auditory perception of temporal order in centenarians in comparison with young and elderly subjects. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 68(3), 373-381.

Kuling, I. A., van Eijk, R. L. J., Juola, J. F., & Kohlrausch, A. (2012). Effects of stimulus duration on audio-visual synchrony perception. Experimental Brain Research, 221(4), 403-412.

Kuroki, S., Watanabe, J., Kawakami, N., Tachi, S., & Nishida, S. (2010). Somatotopic dominance in tactile temporal processing. Experimental Brain Research, 203(1), 51-62.

Liang, W., Zhang, J. Y., & Bao, Y. (2015). Gender-specific effects of emotional modulation on visual temporal order thresholds. Cognitive Processing, 16, 143-148.

Matthews, N., Welch, L., Achtman, R., Fenton, R., & FitzGerald, B. (2016). Simultaneity and temporal order judgments exhibit distinct reaction times and training effects. PLoS ONE, 11(1), e0145926.

McFarland, D. J., Cacace, A. T., & Setzen, G. (1998). Temporal-order discrimination for selected auditory and visual stimulus dimensions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 300-314.

Miyazaki, M., Kadota, H., Matsuzaki, K. S., Takeuchi, S., Sekiguchi, H., Aoyama, T., & Kochiyama, T. (2016). Dissociating the neural correlates of tactile temporal order and simultaneity judgements. Scientific Reports, 6, 23323.

Olatunji, B. O., Ciesielski, B. G., Armstrong, T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Emotional expressions and visual search efficiency:Specificity and effects of anxiety symptoms. Emotion, 11(5), 1073-1079.

Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., & Gabay, S. (2010). Is the future the right time? Experimental Psychology, 57(4), 308-314.

Pérez, A., Penton, L. G., & Valdes-Sosa, M. (2008). Rightward shift in temporal order judgements in the wake of the attentional blink. Psicologica, 29, 35-54.

Pérez, A., García, L., Valdés-Sosa, M., & Jaśkowski, P. (2011). Influence of the learnt direction of reading on temporal order judgments. Psychology, 2(2), 103-108.

Poliakoff, E., Shore, D. I., Lowe, C., & Spence, C. (2006). Visuotactile temporal order judgments in ageing. Neuroscience Letters, 396(3), 207-211.

Pöppel, E., & Artin, T. T. (1988). Mindworks:Time and conscious experience. Boston:Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Roberts, R. D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2008). Task effects on tactile temporal order judgments:When space does and does not matter. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 34(3), 592-604.

Sambo, C. F., Torta, D. M., Gallace, A., Liang, M., Moseley, G. L., & Iannetti, G. D. (2013). The temporal order judgement of tactile and nociceptive stimuli is impaired by crossing the hands over the body midline. Pain, 154(2), 242-247.

Santiago, J., Lupiáñez, J., Pérez, E., & Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (also) flies from left to right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512-516.

Schicke, T., & Röder, B. (2006). Spatial remapping of touch:Confusion of perceived stimulus order across hand and foot. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(31), 11808-11813.

Seibold, V. C., Fiedler, A., & Rolke, B. (2011). Temporal attention shortens perceptual latency:A temporal prior entry effect. Psychophysiology, 48(5), 708-717.

Setti, A., Finnigan, S., Sobolewski, R., McLaren, L., Robertson, I. H., Reilly, R. B., … Newell, F. N. (2011). Audiovisual temporal discrimination is less efficient with aging:An event-related potential study. Neuroreport, 22(11), 554-558.

Shore, D. I., Gray, K., Spry, E., & Spence, C. (2005). Spatial modulation of tactile temporal-order judgments. Perception, 34(10), 1251-1262.

Shore, D. I., Spence, C., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Visual prior entry. American Psychological Society, 12(3), 205-212.

Spence, C., Baddeley, R., Zampini, M., James, R., & Shore, D. I. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments:When two locations are better than one. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(2), 318-328.

Spence, C., & Parise, C. (2010). Prior-entry:A review. Consciousness and Cognition, 19(1), 364-379.

Spence, C., Shore, D. I., & Klein, R. M. (2001). Multisensory prior entry. Journal of Experimental Psychology:General, 130(4), 799-832.

Stetson, C., Cui, X., Montague, P. R., & Eagleman, D. M. (2006). Motor-sensory recalibration leads to an illusory reversal of action and sensation. Neuron, 51(5), 651-659.

Stone, J. V., Hunkin, N. M., Porrill, J., Wood, R., Keeler, V., Beanland, M., … Porter, N. R. (2001). When is now? Perception of simultaneity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 268(1462), 31-38.

Szymaszek, A., Sereda, M., Pöppel, E., & Szelag, E. (2009). Individual differences in the perception of temporal order:The effect of age and cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(2), 135-147.

Szymaszek, A., Szelag, E., & Sliwowska, M. (2006). Auditory perception of temporal order in humans:The effect of age, gender, listener practice and stimulus presentation mode. Neuroscience Letters, 403(1-2), 190-194.

Truong, G., Roberts, K. H., & Todd, R. M. (2017). I saw mine first:A prior-entry effect for newly acquired ownership. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 43(1), 192-205.

Ulbrich, P., Churan, J., Fink, M., & Wittmann, M. (2009). Perception of temporal order:The effects of age, sex, and cognitive factors. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 16(2), 183-202.

Ulrich, R. (1987). Threshold models of temporal-order judgments evaluated by a ternary response task. Perception & Psychophysics, 42(3), 224-239.

van Eijk, R. L. J., Kohlrausch, A., Juola, J. F., & van de Par, S. (2008). Audiovisual synchrony and temporal order judgments:Effects of experimental method and stimulus type. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(6), 955-968.

van Kesteren, M. T. R., & Wiersinga-Post, J. E. C. (2007). Auditory temporal-order thresholds show no gender differences. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25(2), 119-122.

Vroomen, J., Keetels, M., De Gelder, B., & Bertelson, P. (2004). Recalibration of temporal order perception by exposure to audio-visual asynchrony. Cognitive Brain Research, 22(1), 32-35.

Wada, M., Yamamoto, S., & Kitazawa, S. (2004). Effects of handedness on tactile temporal order judgment. Neuropsychologia, 42(14), 1887-1895.

West, G. L., Anderson, A. A. K., & Pratt, J. (2009). Motivationally significant stimuli show visual prior entry:Evidence for attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 35(4), 1032-1042.

Wittmann, M., & Szelag, E. (2003). Sex differences in perception of temporal order. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 96(1), 105-112.

Yarrow, K., Jahn, N., Durant, S., & Arnold, D. H. (2011). Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1518-1531.

Yates, M. J., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2009). Somatosensory prior entry. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(4), 847-859.

Yates, M. J., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2011). Somatosensory prior entry assessed with temporal order judgments and simultaneity judgments. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 1586-1603.

Zampini, M., Bird, K. S., Bentley, D. E., Watson, A., Barrett, G., Jones, A. K., & Spence, C. (2007). "Prior entry" for pain:Attention speeds the perceptual processing of painful stimuli. Neuroscience Letters, 414(1), 75-79.

Zampini, M., Guest, S., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2005). Audio-visual simultaneity judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(3), 531-544.

Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2003). Multisensory temporal order judgments:The role of hemispheric redundancy. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 50, 165-180.

Zampini, M., Shore, D. I., & Spence, C. (2005). Audiovisual prior entry. Neuroscience Letters, 381(3), 217-222.