doi:

DOI:

Advances in Psychological Science (心理科学进展) 2018/26:5 PP.939-950

Collective rituals promote cohesion: Based on action, emotion, and memory


Abstract:
Collective rituals are of great significance to the construction and development of a group.In recent years,foreign psycho-anthropologists have put forward "the theory of modes of religiosity," which elaborates profoundly on the psychological process of collective ritual to produce group cohesion and prosocial behavior.Psychological research into collective rituals focuses mainly on promotion of cohesion,the influence of synchronic movement on prosocial behavior,and the intergroup conflict caused by collective rituals.Some future study topics should be systematic testing and evaluation,such as the psychological mechanism through which collective ritual influences cohesion,and the collective ritual that arouses pleasure emotion.

Key words:collective ritual,cohesion,prosocial behavior,synchronic movement

ReleaseDate:2018-07-02 16:10:50



黄仁宇. (2007). 万历十五年. 北京:中华书局.

王霄冰. (2011). 仪式的建构与表演——2011清明节曲阜祭孔与祭祖活动的人类学考察. 文化遗产, (3), 108-115.

Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group. The Journal of Abnormal & Social Psychology, 59, 177-181.

Atkinson, Q. D., & Whitehouse, H. (2011). The cultural morphospace of ritual form:Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution & Human Behavior, 32, 50-62.

Atran, S., & Henrich, J. (2010). The evolution of religion:How cognitive by-products, adaptive learning heuristics, ritual displays, and group competition generate deep commitments to prosocial religions. Biological Theory, 5, 18-30.

Bell, C. (1997). Ritual:Perspectives and dimensions. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

Bloch, M. (1992). Prey into hunter:The politics of religious experience. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84, 422-428.

Choi, J. K., & Bowles, S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science, 318, 636-640.

Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 369, 20130400.

Clayton, M., Sager, R., & Will, U. (2005, January). In time with the music:The concept of entrainment and its significance for ethnomusicology. In European meetings in ethnomusicology (Vol. 11, pp. 1-82). Romanian Society for Ethnomusicology.

Fessler, D. M. T., & Holbrook, C. (2016). Synchronized behavior increases assessments of the formidability and cohesion of coalitions. Evolution & Human Behavior, 37, 502-509.

Festinger, L. (1962). A theory of cognitive dissonance (Vol. 2). Stanford, CA:Stanford University Press.

Fischer, R., & Xygalatas, D. (2014). Extreme rituals as social technologies. Journal of Cognition & Culture, 14, 345-355.

Fredman, L. A., Buhrmester, M. D., Gomez, A., Fraser, W. T., Talaifar, S., Brannon, S. M., & Swann, W. B., Jr. (2015). Identity fusion, extreme pro-group behavior, and the path to defusion. Social & Personality Psychology Compass, 9, 468-480.

Frost, K. (2016). Cultural and gene-cultural effects and evolutionary dynamics of prosocial rituals (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Davis.

Ginges, J., Hansen, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and support for suicide attacks. Psychological Science, 20, 224-230.

Hagen, E. H., & Bryant, G. A. (2003). Music and dance as a coalition signaling system. Human Nature, 14, 21-51.

Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion:Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution & Human Behavior, 30, 244-260.

Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige:Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution & Human Behavior, 22, 165-196.

Herrmann, P. A., Legare, C. H., Harris, P. L., & Whitehouse, H. (2013). Stick to the script:The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children's imitation. Cognition, 129, 536-543.

Hobson, N. M., Gino, F., Norton, M. I., & Inzlicht, M. (2017). When novel rituals lead to intergroup bias:Evidence from economic games and neurophysiology. Psychological Science, 28, 733-750.

Jones, D. (2013). The ritual animal. Nature, 493, 470-472.

Kapitány, R., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Adopting the ritual stance:The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions. Cognition, 145, 13-29.

Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2011). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:Biological Sciences, 278, 1239-1246.

Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution & Human Behavior, 31, 354-364.

Lakens, D. (2010). Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 701-708.

Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync:Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29, 1-14.

Lang, M., Bahna, V., Shaver, J. H., Reddish, P., & Xygalatas, D. (2017). Sync to link:Endorphin-mediated synchrony effects on cooperation. Biological Psychology, 127, 191-197.

Launay, J., Dean, R. T., & Bailes, F. (2013). Synchronization can influence trust following virtual interaction. Experimental Psychology, 60, 53-63.

Launay, J., Tarr, B., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2016). Synchrony as an adaptive mechanism for large-scale human social bonding. Ethology, 122, 779-789.

Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1998). The relationship between social connectedness and anxiety, self-esteem, and social identity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 45, 338-345.

Legare, C. H., & Herrmann, P. A. (2013). Cognitive consequences and constraints on reasoning about ritual. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3, 63-65.

Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation:The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688-699.

Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2012). Evaluating ritual efficacy:Evidence from the supernatural. Cognition, 124, 1-15.

Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2014). Searching for control:Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy. Cognitive Science, 38, 152-161.

Legare, C. H., & Watson-Jones, R. E. (2015). The evolution and ontogeny of ritual. In The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Vol. 2:Integrations (2nd ed., pp. 829-847). Hoboken, NJ:Wiley.

Legare, C. H., & Wen, N. J. (2014). The effects of ritual on the development of social group cognition. International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, (2), 9-12.

Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142, 351-361.

Lodewijkx, H. F. M., & Syroit, J. E. M. M. (1997). Severity of initiation revisited:Does severity of initiation increase attractiveness in real groups? European Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 275-300.

Lodewijkx, H. F. M., & Syroit, J. E. M. M. (2001). Affiliation during naturalistic severe and mild initiations:Some further evidence against the severity-attraction hypothesis. Current Research in Social Psychology, 6, 90-107.

Lodewijkx, H. F. M., van Zomeren, M., & Syroit, J. E. M. M. (2005). The anticipation of a severe initiation:Gender differences in effects on affiliation tendency and group attraction. Small Group Research, 36, 237-262.

Mann, L., Feddes, A. R., Doosje, B., & Fischer, A. H. (2016). Withdraw or affiliate? The role of humiliation during initiation rituals. Cognition and Emotion, 30, 80-100.

McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying:Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 1-18.

Mcquinn, B. (2012). After the fall:Libya's evolving armed groups. Fortune, 144, 34-35.

Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony's effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 13-20.

Neale, L., Mizerski, R., & Lee, A. (2008, January). Measuring consumer rituals:A marketing application. In Proceedings of the American marketing association winter educators' conference:Marketing the organization and its products and services. Austin, Texas.

Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2013). The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 6-11.

Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 711-729.

Payne, R. J. H. (1998). Gradually escalating fights and displays:The cumulative assessment model. Animal Behaviour, 56, 651-662.

Powell, L. J., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Preverbal infants expect members of social groups to act alike. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, E3965-E3972.

Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.

Reddish, P., Bulbulia, J., & Fischer, R. (2014). Does synchrony promote generalized prosociality? Religion, Brain & Behavior, 4, 3-19.

Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Let's dance together:Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS One, 8, e71182.

Reddish, P., Tong, E. M. W., Jong, J., Lanman, J. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2016). Collective synchrony increases prosociality towards non-performers and outgroup members. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55, 722-738.

Rossano, M. J. (2012). The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 529-549.

Ruffle, B. J., & Sosis, R. (2006). Cooperation and the in-group-out-group bias:A field test on Israeli kibbutz members and city residents. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 60, 147-163.

Ruffle, B. J., & Sosis, R. (2007). Does it pay to pray? Costly ritual and cooperation. The B.E. Journal of Economic Analysis & Policy, 7, 259-277.

Schjoedt, U., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Cognitive resource depletion in religious interactions. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 3, 39-55.

Shaver, J. H., & Sosis, R. (2014). How does male ritual behavior vary across the lifespan? An examination of Fijian kava ceremonies. Human Nature, 25, 136-160.

Sheikh, H., Ginges, J., Coman, A., & Atran, S. (2012). Religion, group threat and sacred values. Judgment & Decision Making, 7, 110-118.

Soler, M. (2012). Costly signaling, ritual and cooperation:Evidence from Candomblé, an Afro-Brazilian religion. Evolution & Human Behavior, 33, 346-356.

Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. R. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity:A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211-239.

Sosis, R., & Ruffle, B. J. (2003). Religious ritual and cooperation:Testing for a relationship on Israeli religious and secular kibbutzim. Current Anthropology, 44, 713-722.

Swann, W. B., Jr., Gómez, Á., Dovidio, J. F., Hart, S., & Jetten, J. (2010). Dying and killing for one's group:Identity fusion moderates responses to intergroup versions of the trolley problem. Psychological Science, 21, 1176-1183.

Swann, W. B., Jr., Gómez, Á., Huici, C., Morales, J. F., & Hixon, J. G. (2010). Identity fusion and self-sacrifice:Arousal as a catalyst of pro-group fighting, dying, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 824-841.

Swann, W. B., Jr., Gómez, Á., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J. F., & Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion:The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 96, 995-1011.

Swann, W. B., Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, Á., Whitehouse, H., & Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal:A theory of identity fusion. Psychological Review, 119, 441-456.

Tajfel, H. (1974). Social identity and intergroup behaviour. Social Science Information, 13, 65-93.

Tambiah, S. J. (1979). A performative approach to ritual. Proceedings of the British Academy, 65, 113-169.

Tarr, B., Launay, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2014). Music and social bonding:"Self-other" merging and neurohormonal mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1096.

Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., & Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bonding. Biology Letters, 11, 1-4.

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). In search of the uniquely human. Behavioral & Brain sciences, 28, 721-727.

Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35-57.

Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept:A social cognitive theory of group behavior. Advances in Group Processes, 2, 77-122.

Uche, C. O., & Atkins, J. F. (2015). Accounting for rituals and ritualization:The case of shareholders' associations. Accounting Forum, 39, 34-50.

Valdesolo, P., & Desteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11, 262-266.

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action:Synchrony promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 693-695.

van Raalte, J. L., Cornelius, A. E., Linder, D. E., & Brewer, B. W. (2007). The relationship between hazing and team cohesion. Journal of Sport Behavior, 30, 491-507.

Waldron, J. J., Lynn, Q., & Krane, V. (2011). Duct tape, icy hot & paddles:Narratives of initiation onto US male sport teams. Sport, Education & Society, 16, 111-125.

Watson-Jones, R. E., & Legare, C. H. (2016). The social functions of group rituals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 42-46.

Watson-Jones, R. E., Legare, C. H., Whitehouse, H., & Clegg, J. M. (2014). Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood. Evolution & Human Behavior, 35, 204-210.

Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27, 34-42.

Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Legare, C. H. (2016). Ritual increases children's affiliation with in-group members. Evolution & Human Behavior, 37, 54-60.

Whitehouse, H. (2002). Modes of religiosity:Towards a cognitive explanation of the sociopolitical dynamics of religion. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 14, 293-315.

Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of religiosity:A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Lanham, MD:AltaMira Press.

Whitehouse, H., & Mcquinn, B. (2013). Ritual and violence:Divergent modes of religiosity and armed struggle. In Oxford handbook of religion and violence. Oxford:Oxford University Press.

Whitehouse, H., & Lanman, J. A. (2014). The ties that bind us. Current Anthropology, 55, 674-695.

Whitehouse, H., François, P., & Turchin, P. (2015). The role of ritual in the evolution of social complexity:Five predictions and a drum roll. Molecular Biosystems, 6, 802-810.

Wiltermuth, S. S. (2012). Synchronous activity boosts compliance with requests to aggress. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 453-456.

Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1-5.

Wood, C., Caldwell-Harris, C., & Stopa, A. (2017). The rhythms of discontent:Synchrony impedes performance and group functioning in an interdependent coordination task. Unpublished manuscript.

Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., … Bulbulia, J. (2013). Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science, 24, 1602-1605.

Xygalatas, D., Schjoedt, U., Bulbulia, J., Konvalinka, I., Jegindø, E. M., Reddish, P., … Roepstoff, A. (2013). Autobiographical memory in a fire-walking ritual. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13, 1-16..